Synthesizing.
Losing
the ability to synthesize.
When
humans first evolved into their present form, they found themselves in
a vicious, nasty world, where eternal vigilence ment the difference
between a short life and a long one. Every sense was a cannel of
precious information that could save those early human lives. By
comparison with this savage world of the past, the modern world, and
especially the western world of the child, is very safe. Because of
this
safety children gradually learn that they do not need to be very
vigilent, and they they do not really nead to finely discriminate
between various minute changes in their environment. Thus they do not
need
to pay close attention to their sensory intake unless there is a large
obvious change. It follows then, that because children no longer need
fine
discrimination to
survive, most of them tend to gradually lose these incredable abilities
in
all their senses.
Synthesizing.
Deep
"True genius resides in the capacity for
evaluation of uncertain, hazardous, and conflicting information."
Winston Churchill
"The person who can combine frames of
reference and draw connections between ostensibly unrelated points of
view is likely to be the one who makes the creative breakthrough."
Denise Shekerjian
Deep understanding.
Deep
knowing and understanding is only possible through a simultaneous
synthesis of various different modes of experience and insight.
Thinking, as the Root-Bernsteins put
it, "...involves or should involve a synergistic interaction
between all our all our sensations and abstract knowledge."
Only some of us do this really well and they are the geniuses of the
world. For this synthesis of knowing and understanding the Root-Bernsteins created an
new word 'synosia'. They say, "Synosia is the natural and
necessary result of imaging, analogizing, modeling, playing and
transforming".
There is a psychological or medical condition known as synesthesia,
where sensation experienced in one sensory mode are overlaid by
experience in another sensory mode. This experience, it is believed,
occurs in about half of all young children and about 5-15 percent of
the world's adults. While it can have very curious disadvantages for
average adults, it could be and is, entirely beneficial for would be
geniuses in increasing their synthesizing ability. While few geniuses
are simultaneously able to use the other tools presented above,
consider how much superior they might have been if they could have.
Synesthesia.
There
is a psychological or medical condition known as synesthesia,
where sensation experienced in one sensory mode are overlaid by
experience in another sensory mode. This experience, it is believed,
occurs in about half of all young children and about 5-15 percent of
the world's adults. While it can have very curious disadvantages for
average adults, it could be and is, entirely beneficial for would be
geniuses in increasing their synthesizing ability. While few geniuses
are simultaneously able to use the other tools presented above,
consider how much superior they might have been if they could have.
The orchestral piece was originally written for
orchestra, piano, pipe organ, choir and light organ literally an organ
that creates light. You may ask what a light organ is, and you would be
perfectly right if you guessed that there is no such thing. So why did
Scriabin compose a piece of music for a fictive instrument? The answer
lies in the concept of “synesthesia”: the perceptual crossover between
senses. Scriabin probably did not believe that one could ever play what
he had composed for the light organ - he simply did not know of any
other way to describe his conception of the music when the “Prometheus”
was played. Although it is still debated whether Scriabin was a "true"
synesthete, the Prometheus is often regarded as one of many examples of
the phenomenon.
Many others have claimed to be so-called
synesthetes, among them Wassily Kandinsky, Vladimir Nabokov, Richard
Feynman and Nikola Tesla. Nabokov, for example, had a quite specific
form of “coloured hearing”, where the sounds of each of the letters of
the alphabet evoked specific hues. As a child, he also sometimes
complained that the numbers and letters on his block were “wrong”. His
mother - also a synesthete - understood and sympathized.
The cross-modality of perception. Synesthesia
stems from the Greek syn-aisthesis ("together-perception"), and is used
for terming the phenomenon where a person has involuntary physical
experience of a cross-modal experience. This means that stimulating a
given sense produces an experience in another sense modality. The most
common example is the “coloured hearing” cases, where a person
experience colours when listening to a particular sound. The
synesthesias are often quite specific and stable, so that separate
instruments might evoke different visual sensations, e.g. their hues
and forms.
How many people experience synestesia? Initially,
this is rather difficult to answer, since it depends on how one choose
to define the phenomenon. On the one hand, one could use a broad
definition, including weaker associations like coupling the vocal “a”
to seeing a red colour. Using what might initially seem a conservative
definition, estimates are still quite high, ranging among children from
40-50%, to 10-20% among adults. On the other hand, others claim that
the phenomenon is much more rare, at a rate at only 1 in 25.000 people.
Unprecedented work has been done by Sean Day, who catalogued 19
different kinds of synesthesia based upon 175 case histories.
Theories on
synesthesia.
In general, there are two major lines of thought
pertaining to Synesthesia. First, there is the theory of synesthetic
metaphors, which claims that Synesthesia is the result of a person’s
vivid imagination. In this view, people that claim to experience
synesthesia take metaphors, like “I see what you are getting at” and
“that color is very loud”, too literally. In the same line of thought,
the theory of linguistic synesthesia claims that synesthesia is
generated through semantic processes and fashioned by time and cultural
elements. One central thesis stemming from this view is the expectation
of cultural differences. That is, if synesthesia is molded from
linguistic acts and cultural influences, any cultures should possibly
reveal differences in the kinds of synesthesias that are expressed
among individuals. Although scientific investigation in this matter has
been rather sparse, it now seems that there are few cultural variations
in synesthesia.
The other type of theory on synesthesia is often
described as “more scientific”, and follows theories from physics and
neurological disorders, as well as the study of effects of psychoactive
drugs. One of the foremost contemporary writers on synesthesia, Richard
E. Cytowic, has proposed a theory of its neural basis. Important to his
work is his definition of the phenomenon, which is comprised of several
pieces, for example that synesthesia is: neither voluntary or
controllable by the subject, or constant - it is usually triggered by
some stimulus “projected” - perceived to take place in the area
immediately surrounding the subject “durable and generic” -
associations between the senses will be constant over time and will
also be relatively abstract.
One of Cytowic’s surprising claims is that
synesthesia is not a result of cortical activity. This is in direct
opposition to theories of the brain basis for normal conscious
sensation. In general, most such theories assume not only a cortical
substrate per se, such as the primary sensory modalities, but also
argue for the necessary role of extensive processing in the frontal
areas of the cortex. Contrary to this, Cytowic cites several pieces of
evidence that synesthesia is accompanied by increased limbic activity -
that is, activity in structures “below” the cortex, often seen as more
primitive structures. At the same time, cortical activity is decreased.
The main reason for Cytowic’s claim is based on an
opposition to what he identifies as the Western notion of a dichotomy
between reason and emotion, and the resulting models where cortex is
placed as being of higher order than the more “primitive” and “lower”
areas, such as the limbic system. By citing Ommaya, a critic of current
brain modeling, Cytowic claims that the corticocentric view of the
brain ignores the fact that "we are irrational creatures by design, and
that emotion, not reason, may play the decisive role both in how we
think and act". The relationship between cortex and the limbic
structures are not one of hierarchy and dominance, but rather of
complex reciprocal communication and interdependence. Thus, if we are
to accept Cytowic's theory of synesthesia, we also are forced to accept
his notion of brain design and functioning.
Specializing of sensory brain paths.
Burt and Smith-Laittan from Cambridge
University have
presented a theory as to why Synesthesia exists. Burt and Smith-Laittan
suggest that, due to the switching on or off of certain genes,
synesthesia may arise from an abnormal failure in the differentiation
between various sensory pathways such as the visual and auditory
signals. During normal development, each brain area that pertains to a
certain sense in adults is specialized and hence differentiated from
other senses. It is possible then, that our infant brains start out
with the sensory pathways being undifferentiated and they gradually
become more differentiated as we get older. This obviously is not the
case with synesthetes. For synesthetes the brain circuitries for, say,
the visual and the auditory pathways are still significantly more
"intermingled", which in turn functions as the basis for the abnormal
sensory integration. If this view is correct a consequence could be
that infants are all alike in all experiencing Synesthesia.
Burt and Smith-Laittan's theory supposes that the
senses though set from birth into certain cortical (and thalamic)
areas, those areas are nevertheless plastic. They note that in infancy,
the brain consists of a multiplicity and abundance of neurons and
connections. During development of an individual, however, they
conjecture that neurons may specialize, creating modules, nodes and
other functional units. It would then follow that neurons that cannot
adapt and make significant specialized connections, would die. Thus,
over time it would seem that conscious perception within a given sense
would become more and more isolated from other senses, and that the
integration of senses would become a more effortful and time consuming
process. For the synesthetes, however, it would seem their brains would
become less specialized and "sensory isolated". This theory would
account for the greater numbers of synesthetes that have been found in
earlier ages of children and the gradual lessening of such numbers as
children grow older.
This development of increasing differentiation in
sensory brain paths may be further understood as being augmented
through the parallel process of inhibition. As we know the frontal
lobes are concerned in part in inhibiting certain actions and changing
the pathways of the brain's circuits as they do. It may well be then,
that the sensory pathways are differentiated in this way by means of
the connectedness between sensory pathways being inhibited. Thus, this
inhibition may well develop as part of the development of the frontal
lobes. Why would a brain the mixing of senses to be counter productive
for evolutionary purposes? It is possible to see many circumstances in
which such mixing of the senses could prove to be both dangerous and
confusing. It follows that synesthesia is probably normal in human
infants and is suppressed as they get older and as normal sensory
experience starts to become dangerous and confusing because of the
mixing.
It may well be, however, that this suppression is
not completely necessary. Clearly many synesthetes are fully functional
humans, and indeed if they have learned to control the phenomenon by
turning it on and off at will, they can be imbued with incredible
advantages.
Types
of synesthesia.
Like so many brain phenomena,
synesthesia seems like hard to believe --- unless you are a synesthete.
But decades of scientific studies have shown that it is real. A better
understanding may shed light on normal conscious perception, and
perhaps even on the minds of highly creative people like Scriabin. If
many children are natural synesthetes, we may also learn more about the
conscious world of childhood. Surprising conditions always reveal
unexpected insights into the brain, its development, and the human
condition.
Numbers and letters
evoking colors. |
121 |
(69%) |
Units of time triggering
colors. |
42 |
(24%) |
Spoken sounds calling up
colors.
|
24 |
(14%) |
General sound evoking
colors.
|
23 |
(13%) |
Musical sounds calling up
colors. |
21 |
(12%) |
Musical notes setting off
colors. |
16 |
(9%) |
Pain evoking colors. |
6 |
(3.4%) |
Odors triggering colors. |
5 |
(3%) |
Personalities evoking
colors. |
5 |
(3%) |
Tastes evoking colors. |
5 |
(3%) |
Sound evoking taste. |
3 |
(2%) |
Sound evoking touch. |
3 |
(2%) |
Vision evoking taste. |
3 |
(2%) |
Touch evoking taste. |
2 |
(1%) |
Sound evoking odor. |
1 |
(0.6%) |
Temperature evoking
colors. |
1 |
(0.6%) |
Taste evoking touch. |
1 |
(0.6%) |
Touch evoking smell. |
1 |
(0.6%) |
Vision evoking touch.
|
1 |
(0.6%) |
Children
and Synesthesia.
Despite different
assessments on the prevalence of synesthesia, one fact is less
disputed; the phenomenon is much more prevalent in children than
adults. But why is this the case? Some speculate that it reflects a
general cognitive development. The famous imagery researcher Alan
Paivio has claimed that children process information mainly by means of
iconic representations, while adults process information in an abstract
manner - in the form of symbolic representations. Building further on
this, Marks has stated that cognitive development come in three stages,
from purely sensory representation of perceptual information, through a
joint sensory/verbal representation, to pure verbal representation.
According to Marks, synesthesia does not vanish with age, but merely
looses its reflexive, sensory character, and is more and more expressed
through language. Support for this thought has been found in studies
showing that adults have weak cross-modal associations, but that they
are weak or come to expression through verbal analogies and metaphors,
and not as “living” images, as found in true synesthetes.
|