Cognitive structure stage four (9-18
Months onward.)
Order from chaos.
The
fourth stage in Greenspan and
Shanker's theory, as depicted in their book
"The First Idea", concerns the creative process; the gaining of greater
understanding of the existence of a self and the possibility of control
over that self, and thus some control over the external world.
This
is accomplished by means of an increasingly developing ability to
distinguish different types of patterns. These patterns are mini maps of
two different but interconnected types. They are patterns of the toddler's actions and emotions
and parallel patterns of the actions and emotions of caregivers. This
site refers to these patterns as reality patterns. Discrimination
between reality patterns and recognition of those patterns enable the
toddler to adapt those patterns to new circumstances.
In order to deal with unforeseen events the child finds that his/her
randomly discovered usable responses do not work well in satisfying
his/her wants and feels the need to alter, change or adapt those
responses to the new situations. Thus the infant has
problems which it tries to solve by means of reducing its own
emotions, suppressing actions and varying existing known patterns of
action.
Complex actions.
At this stage actions become
quite complex. Some action patterns are
added together to create a continuous flow of movement with complex
outcomes. Other action patterns are added together to create incredibly
long chains of emotional
signaling between the infant and its caregivers. Greenspan
and
Shanker explain this as follows:
"...she
learns to engage in a continuous flow of emotional signaling and can
use this ability to solve problems. For example, she may take her
mother by the hand, gesture with her eyes and hands so that her mother
will open the door to the yard, and then point to the swing; or she
takes her dad to the car and shows him that she wants a ride."
The suppression of automatic emotions and actions.
These
long chains of
communication and interaction enable mutual intentions to become clear
without the need of verbal communication, and are themselves achieved
by the infant primarily by means of
terminating or postponing reflex (catastrophic)
emotions and
action templates. By not performing certain actions and suppressing
their emotions infants more easily and increasingly satisfy their needs
and wants. Greenspan and
Shanker explain further:
"...a
child learns how to predict patterns of adult behavior and act
accordingly. She learns, for example, that when her father comes home
and looks grumpy, it's best to stay out of his way. Hide behind the
couch or he will snap at you. The child learns that before her mother
has had her morning coffee she'd better walk and talk softly."
"In daily loving exchanges and
struggles with caregivers, the toddler learns to tame such
catastrophic emotions as fear and rage ... with the more regulated and
interactive use of emotions. Therefore, she learns to modulate and
finely regulate her behavior and moods and cope with intense feeling
states. Anger is explosive in the very young infant, and sadness seems
to last forever and ever. Certain necessary experiences turn these
extreme emotional reactions into feelings and behavior that are finely
regulated and responsive to the situation at hand."
This
dialog of action and repressed action, emotional expression and
suppressed emotion in turn is dependent on caregivers giving immediate
feedback indicating their approval or disapproval of the infant's
actions. These long chains are of two basic types and various
combinations of those types. One type consists of infant actions,
parent expressions of approval or disapproval, action terminated or
continued, approval or disapproval, action modified approval or disapproval
and so on in a long chain. All the while the infant has to reduce the
intensity of emotional expression and terminate or delay reflex
actions. The other type consists of infant expressions of their wants
and parent's guesses and interpretations of what might be meant by the
infant and decisions to give the infant what it wants or deprive the
infant of those wants. These alterations of existing activity programs
create adaptions of old activity programs which are in fact new activity
programs.
Adaptions and the recognition of
patterns.
These adaptions necessitate
the ability to recognize structure in
existing activity programs, structure in emotions, and structure in the
external circumstances in which they occur, and related structure of
outcomes. In other words they require the toddler to recognize patterns
among conjectures. Greenspan
and
Shanker say:
These
savvy adaptions are based on and facilitate, an ability to recognize
patterns. Pattern recognition, which is ideally learned through social
interactions, can then be applied to solve problems in the physical
world as well. The child that doesn't interact, however, won't
experience or fully learn to recognize a broad range of patterns."
Patterns
are essentially groups of conjectures that fit together in a particular
way. They might be conjectures about the child's vocal intonation, or
the fathers attention, the child's own emotional state, or about the
negotiations involved in working together with parents to communicate,
but the pattern is how all these pieces fit together. Greenspan
and
Shanker continue:
"In
other words, pattern recognition involves seeing how the pieces fit
together rather than just being involved in piecemeal
behavior.
Elaborate negotiations or play with others make it possible to
experience the world in larger integrated patterns. Recognizing
patterns helps a toddler predict the behavior of others and adjust her
own."
Problem
solving.
Simple
intention for something to happen is no longer enough the child begins
to
work methodically through or try various alternative patterns to find
solutions to
these problems. This motivation necessitates the distinguishing between
or differentiating between reality patterns to predict the actions of
others and to actively attempt to influence external reality. In this
manner the infant forms patterns of expectation or anticipates the
outcomes of these various created conjectures. At this stage although the toddler is
by now an intentional creature he/she does not
intentionally test these conjectures but rather simply notes the
confirmation or disconformation of such conjectures in his/her continuing experience. Thus although the
conjectures are tested it is accomplished randomly and without intention. Greenspan
and
Shanker explain how increasingly finer control over their emotions
enables toddlers to solve problems and vastly improves their earlier
solutions:
"With
a fine-tuned reaction rather than one that is global or
extreme, the child doesn't have to throw a tantrum to register her
annoyance; she can do it with just a glance and an annoyed look. This
ability comes gradually. Even if a toddler does escalate to a real
tantrum, she will not go from 0 to 60 in one second. Different feelings
from joy and happiness to sadness to anger to assertiveness can become
part of fine-tuned exchanges with patient caring adults."
This
testing is not, however, entirely random. At this stage the child has
the beginnings of what will eventually see him/her becoming a
little scientist. He/she starts trying out various action
patterns and degrees of emotion, which have produced a required result
in one circumstance,
now in a different circumstance. This can be seen as taking already
formed hypotheses and testing them in different circumstances
much
as a scientist would. This is not testing in the sense of trying to
prove the conjecture wrong, but rather an adaptive process where the
solution to one problem can be adapted to another problem.
Complex solutions.
Greenspan
and
Shanker explain how this problem solving leads to increasingly complex
solutions to increasingly complex questions:
"The child also begins to use this
new ability in increasingly complicated situations. Is her mother's
tense face a signal that she is angry with her daughter? The child
starts to use this awareness to respond to people according to their
emotional tones, for example, and to pull away from a situation that
seems undermining. The intuitive ability to decipher human exchanges
and pick up emotional cues before any words have been exchanged becomes
a 'super sense' that often operates faster than our more conscious
awareness. In fact it is the foundation of our social life."
Iteration and theory theory.
Although
at this stage the child has developed very little in the way
of
language many theorists of child development consider that the child by
this time has developed theories about the world and how it works. Such
theories would have to be coded in something other than language such
as an iconic or motor code. This site prefers to call such
structures 'conjectures' because at this stage the child is still
not actively testing such conjectures. However, as this stage develops
children do
begin a process that is very like testing. This process is a seemingly
repetitive cycle
where actions appear to be performed over and over again. However, this
is not
true repetition or true testing but more like variations on a theme.
The child performs
an
action and notes the outcomes of the action and then performs another
action that is similar but not completely the same and notes the
outcomes and then performs another variation and so on. In this way the
child will cycle through many iterations of the original action,
building up a much more comprehensive conjecture defined by limits
within which the conjecture works and limited by conditions within
which the conjecture does not work. In her book "The Scientist in the
Crib" Alison Gropnik points out the systematic way infants examine
objects to discover their properties and gives an example of this early
iterative behavior as follows:
"By
a year or so, they will systematically vary the actions they perform on
an object: they might tap a new toy car gently against the floor,
listening to the sound it makes, then try banging it loudly, and then
try banging it against the soft sofa. By
eighteen months, if you show a them an object with some unexpected
property, like a can that makes a mooing noise, they will
systematically test to see if it will do other unexpected things."
"Babies
also spontaneously undertake solo investigations of the mysterious Case
of the Disappearing Object. Alison once recorded a baby putting the
same ring under a cloth and finding it seventeen times in succession,
saying 'All gone' each time. In our experiments babies often begin by
protesting when we take a toy and hide it. But after one or two turns,
they often start hiding the toy themselves or give the cloth and toy to
us with instructions to hide it again. Eighteen-months-olds, who are
not renounced for their long attention span, will play this game for
half an hour."
"By
the time babies are one or two years old, they will systematically
explore the way one object can influence another object. The babies in
our rake experiments forget all about getting the toy after a trial or
two. They often deliberately put the toy back far out of reach and
experiment with using the rake to draw it toward them. The toy itself
isn't nearly as interesting as the fact that the rake moves it closer.
Situational complexities.
These more defined
conjectures (theories?) enable the infant to deal with increasingly
complex situations. Greenspan
and
Shanker explain:
"The
child also begins to use this
new ability in increasingly complicated situations. Is her mother's
tense face a signal that she is angry with her daughter? The child
starts to use this awareness to respond to people according to their
emotional tones, for example, and to pull away from a situation that
seems undermining. The intuitive ability to decipher human exchanges
and pick up emotional cues before any words have been exchanged becomes
a 'super sense' that often operates faster than our more conscious
awareness. In fact it is the foundation of our social life."
Forming the earliest sense of
self.
Although this stage is still
presymbolic, for the
first time the elements of self previously alluded to previously now
come together to form a whole self entity. Greenspan
and
Shanker continue:
"She also learns to regulate her
moods and behavior and perceive and or organize patterns to form a
more complete self."
"A sense of self begins forming
when a baby organizes her emotions and behavior, such as a few
vocalizations or one or two hand wavings or a few smiles, to a whole
pattern of dozens of exchanges, which she uses to solve problems, she
is learning that she and others can operate in larger chunks or
patterns. This enables her to be even more intentional and to negotiate,
rather than take piecemeal, episodic action. This process happens
step-by-step. When an adult responds reciprocally, the baby makes a
discovery; 'I can make something happen.' This teaches a baby to take
initiative (do something and something happens in return, smiling gets
a smile from mom or dad). ...from this process the infant is beginning
to gain a sense of purpose and will and, very importantly, a sense of
'self' (it's 'me' making something happen 'me' getting that smile or
getting that little red rattle by reaching out 'my' hand). As a
toddler's repertoire of emotional signaling grows richer and she begins
to discern patterns in her own and others behavior, she adds these
observations to the map delineating herself as a person."
Planning.
At
this
stage the beginnings of planning become possible by means of connecting
conjectures together. Greenspan
and
Shanker continue:
"Similarly,
the ability to plan and sequence actions - conducting a five-step
maneuver with a new truck (loading and unloading it, moving it to one
side of the room and then the other) - is also rapidly learned because
of the innovative play where emotional goals are used to guide actions.
As discussed earlier, the toddler is also learning to regulate her mood
and behaviors better because of interactive emotional signaling and, in
this way, is also learning to modulate sensations. She is no longer
likely to become sensory overloaded or underaroused because she is now
able to participate actively in the sensations modulating her. For
example, she can seek out just a bit more sound or touch. She can slow
down an interaction through her expressions, hand gestures, or body
posture if it's becoming overloading."
Multi Tasking.
Also at this stage, being
able to plan and hold more than one action
plan in their mind enables the toddler to engage in more than one
action or thought process at a time. Greenspan
and
Shanker continue:
"New social
skills are also developing at this stage. Social signaling enables the
toddler to handle multiple relationships at the same time, signaling a
mischievous grin to her father and an annoyed look at her
mother. Reading these emotional signals is also part of this process
and it helps the toddler inhibit aggression, cooperate, and altruistic
behaviors."
Imitation.
Although the ability
to imitate seems to be part if an infant's abilities from the moment
they are born at this stage the ability to imitate kicks into high gear
as the toddler becomes able to mobilize actions to imitate others far
more easily and his/her ability to recognize patterns becomes
translated into copying those patterns. Greenspan
and
Shanker continue:
"The ability
to imitate also advances significantly. Now the toddler can copy large
patterns, such as putting on her father's hat, lifting his briefcase,
and imitating him as he walks about the house with a confident stride.
As can be readily imagined, these abilities of social negotiation,
multiple relationships, and rapid learning of whole patterns through
imitation are the foundations for participating in groups. We have
observed toddlers forming friendships by copying each other, following
each other, enjoying some rough-and-tumble play together, and
eventually, by eighteen months of age or so, hamming it up together and
laughing at and with each other."
Correlation.
This
site has labeled this developmental stage as correlations. In
psychology correlation is the interdependence or reciprocal relations
of organs
or functions. In statistics it means the
degree to which two or more attributes or measurements on the same
group of elements show a tendency to vary together. It is essentially
the mutual
relation between two things and this stage is all about relating
conjectures together. These are not the unconscious patterns created at
the previous stage but consciously created patterns that can be recognized and be
made use of to solve problems. This
is the simplest form of intensional creation. This is where toddlers first show
some indication of intentional creativity, just a matter of correlating conjectures
into patterns. These correlations are not at all random, and are
actually based on the fluidity of continuous movement and similarity of
circumstance. In other words 'obvious correlations' not the later
more creative correlations produced by analogy for unconnected items or
that occur serendipitously.
|