Why
do we do things?
Edward Deci
Learning
and self-determination theory. What is
presented in the following is not a full attempt to describe Deci and
Ryan's theory as written by them. It is rather an interpretation of
their theory as regards learning. Although Deci and Ryan say very
little specifically about learning, this site holds that their theory
is all about learning. Their theory is about motivation and nothing can
be learned without motivation. Also their concepts of autonomy,
competence and relatedness, are inexorably entwined in the
understanding of the concept of learning. This page will also provide
some information on other theories but only as they provide a more
integrated understanding of learning. In particular elements of
Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance will be included, of which,
Deci and Ryan's theory of motivation says almost nothing. Cognitive
dissonance will be included because it seems apparent that cognitive
dissonance does support the Deci and Ryan theory. It does this by
simply providing an alternative explanation for why some things work
the way they do. Cognitive dissonance is introduced not to oppose the
Deci Ryan theory but rather to strengthen it.
Self-determination
theory. Edward Deci and Richard Ryan have proposed a theory
of motivation that endeavors to explain all types of motivation under a
single theory. Their new theory is called Self-determination theory or
SDT. The theory developed out of a series of experiments conducted on
groups of people. This research showed that external rewards or threats
of punishment curiously seemed to cause a lowering of the pleasure to
be found in doing certain things, and consequently, a lowering of the
desire to do those things, without the presence of some external reward
or threat. Their theory suggests that there are two types of motivating
forces in humans, those that come from sources that are external to the
human being motivated, and those that come from inside the human. The
motivation that comes from within is intrinsic motivation and that
which comes from external sources is extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic
motivation is held to be the pleasure that is intrinsic in certain
actions. The pleasure is in the action itself. Most of the information
presented here is taken from and interpreted from Deci and Ryan's book
"Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior".
An
aversion to being controlled. In its simplest form this
theory predicts that intrinsic motivation will decline if extrinsic
motivators are applied i.e. threats and rewards. But this not the end
of things. SDT proposes that this is because humans have an aversion to
being controlled and in fact have a deep seated need to choose and
determine their own actions. It asserts that humans have a need for
self-determination that is central to all their motivation.
This
aversion to being controlled, STD shows, has led to two possible types
of behavior when controls (rewards or threats of punishment) are
applied. These behaviors are resistance or compliance. Resistance is
most easy to observe in those who are punished. Children will defy
parents and commit infractions of rules laid down by parents despite
being constantly punished for it. Resistance can also be seen when
people comply. Children often comply with rules to the letter of what
was asked of them, but do not comply in the spirit for which the rule
was intended. This typical of those who have little power like slaves,
and of course children. This resistance probably occurs whenever there
is any inducement or there is any evidence or appearance of control. In
other words regardless of whether the inducement is threat or reward,
even when complying, people do so most of the time with underlying
resistance to perceived control.
Reward
as a deterrent. SDT theory makes some surprising and
anti-intuitive predictions. It predicts that giving people inducements
(either threat or reward) to perform an action, far from making the
action more likely, actually makes the action less likely of being
performed, if there previously existed motivation to perform it.
Inducements tend to be seen as an attempts to control, and for our
purposes, there are two effects of control. One is that when the
controls (the inducements) are stopped or taken away the motivation
they provided goes away also. The other is that the natural inclination
to perform an action without inducement tends to become less as control
is applied or appears to be applied. This reduction of intrinsic
motivation remains even after control is lifted. In this way, the
application of inducements is seen to demotivate as it is withdrawn.
Surprisingly, these effects are the same whether the inducement is a
punishment or a reward. These effects have been born out time and again
over a twenty year period (much to the despair of behaviorists) and are
now fairly well accepted in scientific circles. The logic of this idea
was hard to explain without actually performing an experiment in front
of someone. Deci and Ryan did not have a concise way to explain this
until a friend provided them with a bit of American Jewish folk-law.
The story he provided is presented below in cartoon
form.
Learning
to play music, a story of a girl. Another prediction the
theory makes, is that, if a person is provided with choice in doing
what they are doing they are much more likely to become interested in
it, thus finding and experiencing any inherent pleasure available in
that activity. The following is a story in "Intrinsic and Extrinsic
Motivation a Hierarchical Model" by Vallerand & Ratelle in the
"Handbook of Intrinsic Motivation".
"Take
the case of a sixteen-year-old named Amanda. In general she is the kind
of person who engages in activities because she likes them. She
therefore interacts with friends, plays sports, and goes to school
because of the pleasure inherent in those activities. As a result,
these activities are a great source of enjoyment and satisfaction for
her. This however not is the case when it comes to playing the piano.
Amanda plays the piano because she feels obliged to, certainly not
because she likes it. In fact, she really plays the piano for her
parents (especially her father, a former piano virtuoso). In addition
she feels pressured by her piano instructor who never appears to be
satisfied by her performance. Ever since she started her piano lessons,
Amanda has had Ms. Verkawski, a very controlling woman as an
instructor. The teacher never provides her pupils with opportunities to
experience choice, and she give poor competence feedback. She always
pushes Amanda to play musical pieces that are out of her realm of
skills. Playing the piano is thus associated with the feeling of being
controlled and lacking autonomy and competence. As a consequence, her
performances were never very good and the satisfaction derived from
playing music has been virtually absent.
However
things have started to change recently. For the last two months Ms.
Verkawski has been afflicted with pneumonia and Amanda has been taking
her piano lessons with a new instructor, Mr. McConnell. Amanda's new
teacher is more autonomy-supportive, giving her more freedom to express
herself and letting her explore new avenues. In contrast to Ms.
Verkawski, Mr. McConnell lets Amanda choose among a wide array of musical pieces,
including more contemporary genres. For years, this was what Amanda had
been wanting. More and more, Amanda goes to her piano lessons out of
choice and sometimes experiences pleasure. Consequently, her
performance has improved dramatically and she has started to enjoy
herself more at her lessons.
A
few weeks ago on a Sunday, Mr. McConnell asked Amanda if she would like
to perform in a student recital and asked her to choose a musical piece
to present. She opted for Harry Connick Jr.'s "It Had to Be You", one
of her favorite jazz numbers. Amanda experienced feelings of autonomy because her teacher
provided her with the opportunity to choose whether to participate and
to choose a piece to play. During the weeks prior to the recital,
Amanda devoted considerable time and energy to practicing and
perfecting her number. She was nevertheless very stressed at the idea
of performing in public. On the recital day before it was her turn, Mr.
McConnell told Amanda that she did not have to play if she did not want
to. It relieved her from a lot of stress. She choose to play anyway and
it went well. During her musical number, she felt deeply concentrated
on the rhythms and subtleties of the arrangements in this piece. Even
though she did not put enough emphasis on the decrescendo at the end,
Amanda was happy and satisfied with her performance. After the recital,
Amanda felt inclined to practice more and she became more fully engaged
in piano playing. In fact, at home after the recital Amanda went strait
to the piano to play some more."
The
three most important needs. SDT theory proposes that there
are three important needs and that they are a need for competence
a need for relatedness and a need for self-determination.
These three needs have been used by Deci and Ryan to explain much of
the curious intricacy of human behavior and motivation. Psychologists
and social scientists have been trying to explain this intricacy from
the beginning of those sciences. They explain why we do things. They
explain why we learn.
The
story of Amanda provides a good introductory example of how some
aspects of motivation work according to Self-determination Theory.
Essentially SDT specifies that there are two types of motivation
factors intrinsic and extrinsic. It further specifies, that of the two
types of motivation, intrinsic motivation is far superior in every way.
It is superior not because it is much surer of producing any action
without resistance, although it does that, not because it gives the
motivated person joy and delight, although it does that, but because it
enables the needs of competence, relatedness and self-determination to
be met and satisfied.
Amanda's
motivation. Let us look at what was motivating Amanda:
-
Her
orientation was generally one of intrinsic motivation.
Generally her life was one of intrinsic motivation. She did most things
in her life because she found pleasure in doing them. You might even
say that her orientation in life was essentially one of being
intrinsically motivated. This probably means that her parents for the
most part had been autonomy supportive. They had also, probably sent
her to a school that provided a great deal of choice and was itself
autonomy supportive. It seems strange therefore, that her music teacher
ended up being anything but supportive, but such things can happen.
-
In
music she was mostly extrinsically motivated. She was playing
music, not for the joy of playing music, but in order to have her
father's approval. Perhaps her father had a blind spot and could not
conceive of a person playing music and not enjoying it. He was a
virtuoso after all. She was also probably motivated by guilt, by the
desire to do the right thing and in an effort to repay her parents for
all the autonomy she was allowed outside this one thing.
-
Competence.
Because she was not intrinsically motivated, she had difficulty in
applying her self to the many tasks and skills that had to be mastered
in order to feel competent. Her teacher not only provided no positive
feedback about her competence, but forced her to continually strive to
accomplish tasks beyond her skill level. This insured that she was
never even able to perceive herself as competent.
-
Autonomy.
Although she had plenty of autonomy in the rest of her life, in the
area of learning music she was being completely controlled by her
teacher. Her musical life was painful and gradually becoming more
painful. In this area she could not choose anything, not what to play,
not how to play it. Although she could choose to play, when and where
she was not with her music teacher, she choose not to.
Undoing Amanda's extrinsic
motivation. What does SDT propose to enable Amanda to become
better motivated?
- Autonomy
Support. STD proposes that the most essential thing in any
learning is support for the persons autonomy. This in turn means
providing as much choice in what, how, when and where, as is reasonably
possible in any situation. Improvement in this alone, according to SDT,
will greatly improve the chances of intrinsic pleasure being
experienced, if there is any in the action to experience. This is
essentially what happened to Amanda. Her new teacher provided her with
choice and so she began to experience the intrinsic pleasure of playing
music. She was given choice as to what she would play, and at the
recital she was given the choice of not playing if she so
wished.
- Positive
competence feedback. SDT proposes, that in order to obtain intrinsic pleasure we have
to feel we are competent in performing the action. To some extent we
can judge if we have performed competently or not. But at some new
levels of refining skills, it is difficult for the novice to know how
well he or she has performed. Everyone she asks will have an opinion,
but one of the fundamental jobs of a teacher is to provide some
positive competence feedback to the learner along with the criticism.
Such praise should not be conditional of anything other than increased
competence or it will feel controlling. Although little is said about
the new teacher providing Amanda with praise about her increasing
proficiency it was implied by the suggestion that he thought she had
become skilled enough to perform before an
audience.
- A
secure relational base. We all desire the affiliation with others, the love of others, the
approval of others and the esteem of others. If we do not have those
needs satisfied they also will make it difficult for us to experience
intrinsic pleasure. A secure relational base appears to provide a
needed backdrop—a distal support—for intrinsic motivation, a sense of
security, that makes the expression of this innate growth tendency more
likely and more robust. However, it should be noted, that to experience
the satisfaction of those needs depends, to a large extent, on whether
autonomy support and positive competence feedback are provided by those
others. Amanda did have much better relations with her second teacher
but this was because he provided autonomy support.
Two
ways of reacting to control. The aversion to being
controlled, it was shown, led to two possible types of behavior when
controls were applied. Regardless of whether the controls being used
were rewards or threats of punishment it appears that things can go
either way and often both ways at once.
Resistance.
One way is resistance, where the person being
pressured would end up doing exactly the opposite of what was being
asked. This is the minister's daughter who ends up being wild,
rebellious and an atheist. It is also the basis of reverse psychology,
where the person is asked to perform an activity, so he will perform
the opposite activity. It is the trouble maker in school, the naughty
children, the striking workers, the protesters and the criminals. While
a lot of this clearly understood to be socially aberrant behavior, we
hardly ever think of the other reaction to rewards or threats as being
aberrant.
Compliance.
The other possibility is
of course compliance. What is wrong, you may ask, with doing what you
are told? The answer is that it's not, if we perform the action because
believe it is the right and proper thing to do. But how often is that
true for people? There were a number of experiments performed that show
that most ordinary people will do whatever an authority figure tells
them to do regardless of their beliefs in right and wrong. There was an
experiment performed, where one group was given the task of giving the
other group electric shocks, if they gave a wrong answer. The people
giving the shocks continued to do so, even though the recipients were
screaming and pleading with them not to. The recipients were play
acting, but the people pushing the buttons had no way of knowing that.
Most of the people continued to push the button into the screaming and
pleading stage, because the experimenter simply told them to continue,
when they questioned the advisability of continuing. Something is
surely wrong if people continue doing things they think are wrong just
because someone in authority tells them to. In his book
"Why We Do What We Do" Edward Deci tells this story:
"I
remember years ago when the television networks began the practice of
following a Presidential address with the dissenting views of opposing
senators or congressmen. A friend of mine remarked, 'I don't think it's
right for them to do that.' 'What do you mean?' I replied. 'Dissenting
opinions deserve to be heard.' 'But he's the President,' my friend
protested."
Unauthentic.
This kind of commitment to authority is unauthentic and dangerous and
typifies the compliant attitude. This the sinister nameless authority
that Charles Reich spoke about.
Learning
through competence relatedness and self-determination.
According to SDT we have a need to feel in charge of our selves or to
be self-determined, a need to feel we have the ability to overcome the
challenges of our environment or to be competent, and a need to be
included as part of various wholes, groups or to be related.
- Self-determination.
Self-determination is about our personal
autonomy. We need to feel in charge of our actions where the locus of
causation appears to be inside ourselves. We need to feel we have a
choice. To support us in this way others need to step inside our shoes.
In his book
"Why We Do What We Do" Deci says:
"Providing
choice in the broad sense of that term, is a central feature of
supporting a person's autonomy. It is thus important that people in
positions of authority begin to consider how to provide more choice.
Even in crowded classrooms, fast paced offices, or harried doctors'
offices there are ways, and the more creative one is, the more
possibilities one will find. Why not give students choice about what
field trips to take and what topics to write their papers about, for
example? Why not let the work group participate in the decision of how
to allocate responsibilities? And why not let the patients take part in
planning their treatment regimen? It is not always easy to provide
choice, but it has become increasingly clear that there will be
positive advantages if you do.
The main thing about meaningful choice is that it
engenders willingness. It encourages people to fully endorse what they
are doing; it pulls them into the activity and allows them to feel a
greater sense of volition; it decreases their alienation. When you
provide people choice, it leaves them feeling as if you are responsive
to them as individuals. And providing choice may very well lead to
better, or more workable, solutions than the ones you would have imposed."
- Competence.
Competence is about not only coping with our
environment, but bending it to our will. We need to feel confident in
our ability to do and improve in our doing. This what Edward Deci says
about competence in his book
"Why We Do What We Do":
"The
feeling of competence results when a person takes on and, in his or her
own view, meets optimal challenges. Optimal challenges is a key concept
here. being able to do something that is trivially easy does not lead
to perceived competence, for the feeling of being effective occurs
spontaneously only when one has worked toward accomplishment. Like the
children... [with their] spontaneous, inner desire
to test themselves and the environment, all of us are striving for mastery, for
affirmation of our own competence. One does not have to be the best or
first, or to get an 'A' to feel competent; one need only take on a
meaningful personal challenge and give it one's best."
"...recognizing the importance of competence, many
adults give children challenges and then pressure them to master the
challenges. Especially, if a child shows talent, the challenges can be
great and the pressure intense. But there are two problems in this
approach, even if the adults are well-meaning. First if the challenge
is not optimal - if it is not realistically within the child's grasp -
it will not be motivating. And second, the challenge must be
accompanied by autonomy support rather than control to yield the best
results. In fact when adults are autonomy supportive they will surely
provide optimal challenges, because being autonomy supportive involves
taking the other's perspective. When people do that they will
understand what is optimal for the others. Providing others with
challenges that allow them to end up feeling both competent and
autonomous, will promote in them greater vitality, motivation and
well-being."
-
Relatedness.
Relatedness is about our function as part of
something else our country our ethnic group or of society. We need to
be related to others so we can be part of society etc. Deci and Ryan
put it like this:
"Although autonomy and competence have been found
to be the most powerful influences on intrinsic motivation, theory and
research suggest that relatedness also plays a role, albeit a more
distal one, in the maintenance of intrinsic motivation... During
infancy, intrinsic motivation is observable as exploratory behavior,
and attachment theorists suggested that exploration is more robust when
infants are securely attached to a parent. Studies of mothers and their
young children show that maternal autonomy support as well as the
security of attachment presumed to be fostered by it are both
associated with exploratory behaviors.
Indeed, across the life span, SDT hypothesizes that
intrinsic motivation will be more likely to flourish in contexts
characterized by a sense of secure relatedness. For example, Ryan and
others showed greater intrinsic motivation in students who experienced
their teachers as warm and caring. People often engage in intrinsically
motivated behaviors (e.g., playing solitaire, hiking) in isolation,
suggesting that relational supports may not be necessary as proximal
factors in maintaining intrinsic motivation. Instead, a secure
relational base appears to provide a needed backdrop—a distal
support—for intrinsic motivation, a sense of security that makes the
expression of this innate growth tendency more likely and more robust."
Why
do we do things? Why do we learn?
-
Fear
of consequences if we do not. Some things we do, such as
learning, we do because we are afraid of what will happen if we do not
do those things. This avoidance behavior.
If
we do things entirely because we are afraid of punishment or because we
wish to avoid unpleasantness this falls under the heading of pure
extrinsic motivation. It is hardly ever the case that we do things for
that reason alone. The action may bring us closer to satisfying some of
our biological needs, it may involve some intrinsic reward, or there
may be some external reward involved. If the fear is strong however it
will overcome the other needs and present itself as fairly pure
extrinsic motivation.
-
Anticipation
of a reward. Some things, such as learning, we do because we
anticipate a reward will be forthcoming from the environment if we do
those things. This the so called conditioned behavior.
If
we do things entirely because we anticipate reward or because we
anticipate increased external pleasantness this falls under the heading
of pure extrinsic motivation. It is hardly ever the case that we do
things for that reason alone. The action may bring us closer to
satisfying some of our biological needs, it may involve some intrinsic
reward, or there may be some external punishment involved. If the
reward is strong and salient however, it will overcome the other needs
and present itself as fairly pure extrinsic motivation.
-
Fear
of feeling bad if we do not. Some things, such as learning,
we do because we are afraid of how we will feel if we do not do those
things. This avoidance behavior.
If
we do things because we are afraid of how we will feel if we do not,
this all about our sense of self or our self concept. If we do things
that are in conflict with what we understand our self to be, we feel
bad and we can anticipate that feeling. Some people call this
inconsistency dissonance, so the anticipation of it would be called
anticipated dissonance. Others have called it a need for self
affirmation and still others a need for self consistency.
-
Because it is the right thing to do according to
our inner self concept. Some things, such as learning, we do because it is
the right thing, for us, to do them. This a type of motivation for
which we have no real name. It is also a very relativistic experience
because what is right for me may not be right for you. The criminal who
does something society ascribes as wrong, may not feel it wrong
himself. The looser who performs badly does not consider that is
inconsistent with his idea of his self. What is right for the criminal
and the looser is to be a criminal and a looser.
If
we do things because its the right thing to do this also about the need
to have a consistent concept of self. But it is not about being afraid
of feeling bad if we do something, that is not consistent with our
concept of self. It is hard to see exactly what it is. Perhaps there is
some pleasure in doing things that are consistent with our concept of
self, even if the actions are not in themselves intrinsically
pleasurable. There was once even a city state and a philosophy that
exemplified this idea. The city state was called Sparta and the
philosophy was called stoicism.
-
To
satisfy our biological needs. Some things, such as learning,
we do because it is essential to placate the biological needs we were
born with, which we anticipate will be automatically rewarding in
themselves. This intrinsically motivated behavior.
If
we do things because we want to satisfy biological needs we do it in
order to obtain a sensual experience. This can be and often is
inexorably entwined with extrinsic reward. If we want to eat we usually
have to bargain with others about it. Perhaps it requires some barter
exchange or money. All can be used as extrinsic reward. Perhaps we give
others the extrinsic reward of money so they will give us food or
perhaps they give us the extrinsic reward of food so we will give them
money. It can be either, or neither. It all comes down to a matter of
perception. Food tastes best when we feel we have earned it or given
what was due for it. The same is true of safety, love or esteem. Only
when we have earned them do they feel truly satisfying. Thus we do
things in order to feel autonomous, we do things in order to feel
competent, and we do things in order to feel related to others.
-
Belief
that it will be rewarding in itself. Some things, such as
learning, we do because we anticipate that we will experience joy in
doing them. This also intrinsically motivated behavior which not only
includes satisfying biological needs but also as set of aesthetic needs
which are also biological but often require considerable activity
before they can be experienced.
If
we do things because we think they will be rewarding in themselves, we
do it for what people usually understand to be very high minded values.
We do it to help or entertain others that we are bound to in some way.
We can do it for family, for friends, for our county for our race and
for mankind. We can do it even in the name of all living things. It
includes what is understood to be good, or creative, useful, etc. etc.
At its best it is overcoming a challenge, learning, increasing,
improving, becoming more than what we were, of actualizing our
potential. For self-actualized people there is no conflict between
these values and their own concept of self. This level of regulation is
also very much about doing things in order to feel competent,
autonomous, and related to others. For Deci and Ryan it is the very
stuff of life itself.
Some
actions are intrinsically rewarding and some are not. Those
actions that are intrinsically rewarding are mostly all the things that
satisfy our biological needs. And what are our biological needs? While
Deci and Ryan tend to lump these under the umbrella of a 'need to
relate to others', I prefer the clarity of Maslow's listing of
biological needs.
Physiological
needs. They are feelings of satisfaction obtained
from eating, drinking, breathing, being warm when its cold and cool
when its hot, having sex etc.
Safety
needs. They are the feelings of making ourselves safe and
feeling safe and secure.
Love
and belonging needs. They are feelings of being loved and as
if one belongs or is affiliated with some group.
Esteem
needs. They are feelings as if one is held in high esteem and
the feelings of having high self-esteem.
Meta
or aesthetic needs. Then there are those biological needs
that Maslow calls meta needs being needs and aesthetic needs. They are
the needs that we can take or leave. We do not suffer a deficiency if
we do not satisfy them, but they can be very powerful in achieving a
feeling of immense pleasure, if they are satisfied. They are acting in
ways to achieve something, to create something to do good things which
help others, to accomplish something, to feel competent and skilled in
what we are able to do, to learn, and to have actualized some potential
in ones self.
Are
we always intrinsically satisfied when performing innately pleasurable
actions? Although some actions are intrinsically rewarding
that is not always the reason why we do them nor are we always the
recipient of pleasure from that intrinsic reward. One would think that
the reason we perform any one of these actions would be because of
these intrinsic satisfiers. Alas this not always the case. The problem
is that, even though, each and every action that satisfies any one of
these needs is intrinsically rewarding in and of itself, if we are
offered extrinsic reward the intrinsic pleasure tends to diminish or
disappear entirely. In fact, just because it is possible to experience
the exquisite pleasure of intrinsic reward does not ensure that it will
be experienced.
What
happens is, when others offer external rewards or punishments to
motivate us to perform these actions, this acts as a damper on these
intrinsic rewards causing them to become muffled or not in fact become
active at all. You might, as commonsense would have it, expect that if
you offered someone a reward for being creative, or eating his food, or
for accomplishing something, or for feeling safe, or for feeling loved
or for being held in high esteem, or for doing a good deed, that this
would simply add to that someone's pleasure. This not the case. What
happens is the intrinsic pleasure that would normally accompany these
actions is diminished. We no longer perform the actions because of
these intrinsic pleasures, but rather, because we expect external
reward for performing them. Likewise if we perform actions that are
innately intrinsically rewarding in themselves, when there is also a
threat of punishment if we do not perform them, the intrinsic pleasure
innate in those actions will gradually diminish till it is extinguished.
Of
what practical use is this knowledge? "That's all very well",
I hear you saying, but what practical use is it? "How does it help me
to get my children to learn?" the parents say. "How does it help me to
motivate my pupils?" the teachers say. "How does it help to motivate my
employees?" the business executives say. And the answer is this, "You
can't. You are in fact asking the wrong question". The answer
after 20 years of meticulous experiments on groups of human beings,
after innumerable observations of children in schools and employees in
the workplace, is simply that no matter how much we might want to
motivate others, it cannot be done. There is no way
to keep a child or adult doing what we ask, when the extrinsic
punishments or rewards are withdrawn. It cannot be done because they
are already motivated and if we are not careful our own actions will
interfere with that motivation and tip it in the wrong direction.
This
idea is not so new in itself. Maria Montessori knew it intuitively,
John Holt arrived at it through his experience as a teacher, John Dewey
knew it, as did Abraham Maslow and Carl Rogers. What was new was that
now there was reproducible, irrefutable scientific proof. Deci and Ryan
had not only shown what could not be done, but also what can or could
be done. Here it is as succinctly put as possible.
The
only thing we can do is to provide an opportunity, an environment, in
which children, employees and pupils, will be willing to, and want to,
motivate themselves.
Why
then have these findings not yet been fully accepted and implemented?
The answer is the same as it is for any scientific discovery. A new
paradigm is formed, which many can test for holes. It takes time and a
new generation of people growing up before it can be fully accepted,
and this will no doubt be little different.
Note
in passing. Although Deci and Ryan's and most others doing
these experiments did so using intrinsic motivation from the meta level
of Maslow's hierarchy. It is, I believe fairly obvious, it would work
just as well with the lower needs. Here are a few questions, to which
my personal answer would be no. If someone paid you to eat a delicious
meal would it taste as good as if they hadn't? If someone threatened to
punish you if you did not drink something that you normally like to
drink would it taste as good? If someone paid you to have sex with a
beautiful woman would it feel as good as if they had not? If someone
paid you to make yourself safe would it feel as good as if they had
not? If some one paid you to be friends with someone would it feel as
good a friendship as where they had not? If someone threatened to
punish you if you did not get the attention and love of a particular
girl would it feel as good as if you had not been threatened? What if
someone had paid you to cause someone to hold you in high esteem, would
that esteem feel as good?
The
need for autonomy. Let us look at what Deci and Ryan
discovered. They postulated a need for autonomy based on the finding
that both extrinsic reward and threat of punishment appeared to cause
intrinsic motivation to diminish. Therefore controlling contexts, in
terms of the theory would obviously cause personal autonomy to appear
to diminish, and thus in turn cause the diminishment of intrinsic
motivation. It followed then, that any circumstances that caused
feelings of being controlled, would likewise cause a drop in intrinsic
motivation, and they did.
The
following contexts were all found to cause intrinsic motivation to
diminish.
-
Extrinsic
Reward Generally decreases intrinsic motivation.
-
Threat of
Punishment Generally decreases intrinsic motivation.
-
Surveillance
Generally decreases intrinsic motivation.
-
Deadlines Generally decrease intrinsic
motivation.
-
Evaluation
Generally decreases intrinsic motivation.
-
Imposed
Goals
Generally decrease intrinsic motivation.
-
Competition
Generally decreases intrinsic motivation.
Temporary
compliance. It was beginning to appear that the whole arsenal
that parents, teachers and business executives used to motivate those
in inferior positions, were in fact doing just the opposite. They were
causing temporary compliance and the appearance of self motivation, but
in reality, where it really counted, in intrinsic motivation, they were
actually damaging those under them and making them less capable. In his
book
"Why We Do What We Do" Deci puts it like this.
"With
our children, students and employees we typically hope that the desired
behaviors will continue even if we are not there...[to reward
them]. We'd like them to keep learning, to keep producing, to
keep doing their share of the housework, and the question we face is how to promote such persistent
self-direction rather than the irresponsibility or alienation that
seems so prevalent in today's world."
About
money. Deci and Ryan used money in many of their research
experiments because they knew that money was the most important
extrinsic reward. They do not deny the importance of money not even as
a motivator. The following is from Deci's book
"Why We Do What We Do":
"Obviously,
money constitutes a powerful force. Certainly there can be no doubt
that it motivates. One need only look around (even at oneself) to see
how willing people are to engage in a wide range of activities for
money. They drag themselves to work at jobs they hate, because they
need the money. They get hooked on gambling, sometimes loosing
everything they own, because of an irrational belief that they will hit
the big one. They take on extra assignments that unduly stress them,
perhaps to the point of making them sick, because of extra money. And
they engage in a variety of nefarious activities that promise handsome
rewards. Sure, money motivates, but that is not the point. The point is
that while money is motivating people, it is also undermining their
intrinsic motivation and, as we would later discover, having a variety
of other negative effects as well."
"Money
is the medium of exchange in all modern economic systems, so monetary
payments rewards have to be dispensed. But there are better and worse
ways of doing that. It is better, for example, not to think about
rewards as a way to
motivate people. Rewards are part of the work contract, so you would
not have workers without rewards. But research suggests that, to the
extent that rewards are "used" for any function other than retaining
workers, it ought be merely to acknowledge or signify a job well done.
Rewards can be used to express appreciation, but the more they are used
as motivators - like the bonus plan in the publishing house - the more
likely it is they will have negative effects."
The
cost of diminishing intrinsic motivation. Deci and Ryan and
many others through their research findings gradually built up a
picture of what it meant to diminish intrinsic motivation. They found
consistently that people with reduced intrinsic motivation had:
- Less
of interest and vitality. Loss of interest in new things to
learn, loss of interest in the things they were already interested in,
loss of interest in learning, loss of the vital spark of life that
makes us more than pawns carrying out the desires of others, these were
the most terrible costs of decreased intrinsic motivation.
- Less
creativity. Through the work of Teresa Amabile it was
clearly shown that loss of intrinsic motivation led to loss of
creativity. Loss of intrinsic motivation meant that the artists used
less varied materials, they produced less varied motifs, they produced
work that had technically less quality and they showed less
originality. They were basically less able to produce real works of art.
- Less
conceptual understanding of material learned. A clear
pattern emerged that people who were extrinsically motivated to learn
any material that required conceptual understanding. They would tend to
use, so called, 'rote learning' in the form of cramming, which proved
of little use in answering exam questions, other than to regurgitate
answers whole. Conceptual understanding was clearly not taking
place.
- Less
retention of knowledge. While it was found that
extrinsically motivated people could very effectively memorize rote
material for a test better than those who were intrinsically motivated,
their ability to retain this material was far less. It was found that
this material was generally dumped by the mind after the test was over.
Ryan likened this to a core dump by a computer.
- Less
academic achievement. With the loss in conceptual
understanding and the loss of retention of knowledge, it should not be
surprising to find that less intrinsic motivation led to less academic
excellence.
- Less
ability to solve problems. Even when insuring that the
information and understanding of a problem was equal, it was found that
that those who were not intrinsically motivated had far less capacity
to solve any sort of problem. Perhaps this because of the connection
between problem solving and creativity.
- Less
quality of task performance. Taking into account all of the
above ability reductions, it should not come as a surprise that any
task performance might tend to suffer when intrinsic motivation was
reduced, and this certainly is the case.
How
can we implement an environment that encourages self motivation?
Well, first we need to know exactly how and why people really do come
to do the things other people want them to. What Deci and Ryan did next
was try to see how they could provide circumstances, that would
prevent, or at least ameliorate the apparent diminishment of autonomy.
They reasoned that if they provided choice within the contexts of
experiments, where people were feeling controlled, then that should to
some extent prevent the loss of apparent autonomy, and in doing so
should make the loss of intrinsic motivation less acute.
The
results were even better than expected. The inclusion of even a small
amount of choice seemed to prevent intrinsic motivation from
diminishing very much.
Other
experiments followed, where it was discovered that all the above
mentioned controlling contexts could be modified to be or appear to be
less controlling, and thus could be far less likely to reduce intrinsic
motivation. In each case it was found, that the persons own view of
what was happening was all important. The views of others concerning
their autonomy, competence or interrelatedness counted for nothing if
the person felt that their own actions were being controlled or that
their actions were ineffective and incompetent.
- Extrinsic Reward. It was found that rewards, if
they were perceived to be given as acknowledgments of competence, and
not as part of some condition, would not in fact, cause intrinsic
motivation to decrease. Also rewards given unexpectedly were not found
to be damaging to intrinsic motivation. Praise in
particular, was in fact found to be very effective in increasing
intrinsic motivation, if it was confined to the quality of work and the
improvement that had been accomplished. In other words, if the praise
was truly informational feedback, and not seen as an attempt to
control, it increased rather than decreased intrinsic motivation.
-
Threat of
Punishment. It was found that a small amount of threat could be
used to jump start people performing some action, if no interest
existed for its performance. This threat could and should however, be
stopped if any interest began to appear. There is no loss of intrinsic
motivation because none previously existed.
-
Surveillance.
It
was found that the presence of others would not decrease intrinsic
motivation if the people were not observing, but rather working on
their own independent tasks and seemed involved in what they are doing.
Again it depends on whether there appears to be an attempt to control
or not.
-
Deadlines.
It
was found that deadlines, if self imposed, or part of a communal
crisis, could be used to produce quicker work without any reduction in
intrinsic motivation. But any indication that the deadlines were
imposed to manipulate would cause intrinsic motivation to fall.
-
Evaluation.
It
was found that both praise and criticism, could be performed in a way
that did not cause intrinsic motivation to diminish. Criticism in the
form of negative feedback, if the person was not overly sensitive to
criticism, could be entirely informational and combined with an
indication of improvement to increase intrinsic
motivation. Again it is all a matter of whether there is
perceived control or not.
-
Imposed Goals.
It
was found that imposed goals could, if provided with sufficient choice,
be accepted as being within the bounds of volitional autonomous
activity, and thus not cause much in the way of loss in intrinsic
motivation. It would thus not be seen as controlling. Self
imposed goals likewise do not cause a decrease in intrinsic motivation
if one is not forcing one's self but rather acting from an integrated
self.
-
Competition. It was found that
competition was not likely to cause a decrease in intrinsic motivation
if the accomplishment the person was seeking was one of improving and
thus performing excellently, as opposed to winning or beating the other
person. When competition was with one's self, and concerned itself only
with one's own improvement, it could in fact be found to increase
intrinsic motivation.
Socialization, learning society's regulations
and social norms.
Activities
that are not intrinsically interesting. Deci and Ryan began
to wonder how activities that were not intrinsically interesting in
themselves could be learned at all. Clearly they were learned, so they
theorized some system must be in place. The answer to how non
intrinsically interesting regulations were assimilated was to be found,
they believed, in the needs for relatedness, competence and
self-determination themselves.
Limits
and appropriateness. Other activities were also a problem. It
often was not so much that the activities were in themselves
unpleasant, it was more a matter of not doing things that were in fact
intrinsically pleasurable. It was often a matter of forgoing or putting
off till a more appropriate occasion activities that in fact were
intrinsically pleasurable. It was necessary to set limits and contexts
for appropriateness. Deci and Ryan hypothesized that there should be
ways of presenting the regulations needed for socialization, in such a
way as to not cause either unquestioning compliance on the one hand, or
resistance on the other.
Socialization,
demands as invitations. It occurred to them that demands for
behavior did not have to be presented as demands, but rather could be
presented as invitations. The regulations presented in this way, they
reasoned, could have a chance of being perceived as not being
controlling and thus acceptable to the self concept, sufficiently so as
to start a process of internalization. The factors included in this
manner of presentation were acknowledgment of possible dislike, the
rationale or reasons why actions were to be performed, avoidance of
controlling language and manner, provision of options within the
regulations, and the obtaining of prior agreement.
- Acknowledgment. People are
much more likely to be willing to internalize regulations and not
venture beyond the limits being set, if it is acknowledged the that
they might not wish to do so. This provides both information and
indicates acceptance helping to satisfy both the need for competence
and the need for relatedness. It greases the path to internalizing
regulations.
- Rationale. The first and
most essential way in which any action can be autonomy supported, is to
provide information as to why there is need for the limitations or
action. People need to know why such limitations or actions are
necessary. Children are always asking why they should do something, but
parents are most often unable or unwilling to tell them why. Yet, the
reason why, is what enables the child's desire to be part of something
greater than themselves, to facilitate in the internalization of
regulations. Reasons give information that enables competence and also
enabling choice which enables self-determination. How can you choose to
internalize a regulation if you don't know why you are doing it?
- Avoid controlling language and other
controlling signs. Obviously, any kinds of signs that would
indicate that control is being applied are to be avoided. One helpful
way is to refrain from using controlling language. Words like 'should',
'must', 'have to' and 'be good' are not helpful in being autonomy
supportive, and thus, it is best not to use them when suggesting
regulations. Perception of not
being controlled is absolutely essential in feeling self-determined.
- Provide choice. Any choice,
the smallest amount of choice, will be helpful in enabling people to
feel autonomous. Setting limits by definition means reducing options,
but it does not require that there be only one allowable option. It was
found, that if we want regulations to be internalized, the more choice
that was allowed within the set limits, the more the feeling of self
determination and the more the internalization proceeded.
- Contract. Contracts are a
very good way of setting limits, or encouraging actions, for children
old enough to understand what a contract is. Contracts, agreements, not
only allow those in an inferior position to take part in the
formulation of the regulation to be internalized, but also provide
choice in acceptable and equitable punishment if the contract or
agreement is violated. The commitment and responsibility that is thus
invoked, also is helpful in facilitating the internalization of
regulations.
Relating
the theory to extrinsic motivation. For Deci and Ryan
extrinsic motivation is very volatile and requires very careful
handling. They noticed that socialization seemed to work best
when the least possible extrinsic motivators were used. It is not
enough for people to do the things required by society. It is necessary
that people want to do those things. Deci put it like this:
"The
real job involves facilitating their doing the activities of their own
volition, at their own initiative, so they will go on doing the
activities freely in the future when we are no longer there to prompt
them."
Are
humans passive or proactive creatures? Whether we under stand
humans as coming into the world as little barbarians who have to be
tamed into being human, as passive blank slates that have to be molded
into being human, or as proactive inquisitive creatures who come into
the world seeking how to be human, is very germane to how they need to
be socialized. For Deci, Ryan and this site the answer to this very
clear. Everything we have observed about humans, and about infants in
particular, tells us that humans are proactive beings, seeking how to
be more human, from our first moments.
The
only problem with this idea, and the reason others tend to dismiss it,
is the huge numbers of humans, who end up being badly socialized, unfit
for human society, the sociopaths. The answer is the genetically
supplied mechanism that that allows humans to become socialized, relies
on infants and ultimately adults being supplied with certain
psychological conditions, in order to grow into full humanity. Just as
we need adults to feed us, protect us from harm and communicate with us
when we are young so we can grow into being fully human, we also need
to feel self-determined, competent and related to other humans, in
order for us to become properly socialized. In his book
"Why We Do What We Do" Deci says:
"How
can this be, if as I suggested, people are naturally motivated to
internalize aspects of their social world that are useful for their
social functioning? In other words, why do so many people fail to
become willing to do
activities that are good for them? To understand this seeming paradox,
we must go back to the dialectic - the interaction between the active
organism and the social
context that can either support or thwart what the organism would be
inclined to do naturally.
If
you put a rooted avocado pit in a pot of earth it will probably grow
into a tree, because it is in the nature of avocados to do that. It
happens naturally. But not all pits become trees; some shrivel and
decompose. They fail to thrive because the climate is inadequate or the
necessary nutrients are lacking. They need sun; they need water; and
they need the right temperatures. Those elements do not make the trees
grow, but they are nutriments that the developing avocados need, that
are necessary in order for the avocados to do what they do naturally.
In
the same way, developing human beings need psychological nutriments to
do what they do naturally. To internalize and integrate aspects of
society that are important for their own success in life within
society, they must experience satisfaction of their basic psychological
needs - for autonomy, competence and relatedness - within the social
context that provides structures to be internalized."
Socialization
through internalization. The solution Deci and Ryan arrived
at was, that people were initially motivated to internalize regulations
through the need for relatedness, which enabled them to be part of
groups such as the family and society. The non controlling method of
presentation of regulations, was essentially providing information to
facilitate a process of motivation, that was already in place. The only
effective way to facilitate this motivation for socialization, was to
provide a context within which people could and would choose to
internalize these regulations. Once internalized the regulations became
part of the person, which would enable that person to motivate himself
to perform these activities that were perhaps not in themselves
intrinsically pleasurable.
Deci
and Ryan reasoned that in order to be accepted as part of the various
groups we belong to, we are motivated to find out how to behave in ways
that are acceptable to those groups. This they hypothesized, would not
involve simply mimicry, although that could play a part, but would
involve them accepting a roll in the community and contributing to that
community. Initially children had to find ways of becoming acceptable
to and contributing to the family. Gradually they moved out and had to
become acceptable to and contributing to larger and larger communities.
Sometimes the values and expectations of one community such as the
family might conflict with those of a group of peers. If, however,
families had passed on regulations in the most effective manner this
kind of conflict would not occur. Deci explains it as follows in
"Why We Do What We Do".
"All children face
the challenge of responding to society's beckoning without being
overwhelmed or suffocated by it. They must find a way to become related
to the social world while also achieving a sense of integrity within
themselves. To a great extent, whether these young people are
successful in their attempts to be authentic and responsible depends on
the quality of their socializing environment. It depends on whether the
socializing environment provides the nutriments they need. It depends
on whether the socializing environment allows them to them to satisfy
their psychological needs while internalizing the values and
regulations in question."
But
what was the most effective manner of passing on these regulations?
While it is very difficult to escape situations where extrinsic threat
and reward are expected, it was clear from the research that threat and
reward should be used in the least amount possible for a given
situation in order to achieve the best results.
The
less extrinsic motivation the better. Curiously and
anti-intuitively Deci and Ryan discovered through experiments that mere
request was more effective than making demands, threat of punishment or
giving rewards. Less was somehow better. This operated through a number
of cue or signals that
were related to their three basic needs for self-determination,
competence and relatedness.
5
states of motivation. Deci and Ryan's theory actually
proposes that there are different states of being motivated and
depending on the quality of need satisfaction and the quality of
presentation of regulations. They postulated five distinguishable
states of being motivated and a sixth state of being not motivated
which they called amotivation. These five motivational states are:
-
Intrinsic
regulation. We perform actions because those actions are
innately rewarding.
-
Integrated
regulation. We perform actions because we
feel those actions are part of our concept of our
self.
-
Identification
regulation. We perform actions because we identify with those
actions. We have accepted those actions as being part of our self
concept but they are still in conflict with certain aspects of our
integrated self.
-
Introjected
regulation. We perform actions because we have accepted those
actions as being consistent with that part of our self concept which
tells us what is right. They are accepted in principle, but are still
completely at odds with many elements of our existing self concept. It
is as if they are swallowed whole, without any modification for
assimilation and without the self concept modifying itself to
accommodate them.
-
Compliance
with external regulation. We do things in order to obtain
extrinsic rewards or in order to avoid punishment.
The
introduction of new regulations where intrinsic motivation already
exists.
How
the states work. I don't think Deci and Ryan intended that
these states were separate, but were rather like points on a gradient
or a graph. Basically motivation is a continuum which depends on two
aspects the amount of pleasure intrinsic in any action and those
external forces of threat or promise of reward. Let us look first at
those actions that are intrinsically rewarding in and of themselves.
-
Ideal
motivation, intrinsic motivation. The ideal motivation is
pure intrinsic motivation. In this situation there is free choice, and
we choose to do something because we anticipate that it will be
pleasurable to do so. In this ideal situation there are also no
external threats preventing you from doing that something, and there
are no external rewards offered for doing that something. However, as
stated, this an ideal situation that rarely, probably never happens.
The fact is that everything we do no matter how pleasant has baggage.
Every action involves some unpleasantness at least at first.
Eating
a delicious meal is obviously intrinsically rewarding, yet there are
undoubtedly actions that lead up to the action and follow from the
action, that are initially unpleasant. For instance, before a meal can
be consumed it must be prepared or cooked. This not obviously something
that is intrinsically rewarding, but it can become intrinsically
rewarding, as it has the potential to become creative. The meal cannot
be cooked unless someone has gone out and bought the items needed for
the meal. Shopping is something that is also not obviously
intrinsically rewarding, but it too, has the potential to become so, if
performed creatively. Finally at some point in this modern world money
has to be spent on items, so the items needed may be bought. Giving
away money is not naturally a pleasant action, but it can become so, if
we spend the money creatively to create something.
Social
contagion. In terms of Deci and Ryan's theory, how we come to
try new activities is through social contagion.
Social contagion predicts that people attribute certain motives to
other people through certain cues in the other people's behavior. These
observations, they believe, reveal those other people's true motives.
If people perceive other people's actions as being intrinsically
motivated, then social contagion predicts that the original people will
want to try those actions, to see for themselves. Of course the innate
intrinsic pleasure of the activity is then able to take hold and keep
them performing those actions. Although some parts of the activity may
have been initially perceived as being boring or unpleasant, those
parts can become pleasant through association or if there is hidden
innate pleasure in performing them that can be enabled. When a person
acts as if they are getting intrinsic pleasure from performing an
action they give off cues to this effect. This in turn induces an
aspiration by those observing them to anticipate the same kind of
intrinsic pleasure. This in turn motivates them to imitate the actions
of that person. This is called social contagion because there are
always more people picking up on cues than being observed. For
instance, suppose two or more people observe a person performing an
action, and because the action seems to be intrinsically motivated, it
induces those two or more people to be motivated to imitate the
original action. If those two or more people are then observed giving
off cues of being intrinsically motivated by four or more people, they
in turn become intrinsically motivated. So it follows that
intrinsically motivated people can grow in numbers just like an
epidemic.
-
Where
both intrinsic and extrinsic motivators are in consonance.
For intrinsic and extrinsic motivators to be acting in the same
direction means, not only is there intrinsic pleasure innate in the
action, but there is and external reward offered for performing the
action as well, or a threat in force if the action is not performed.
This situation of having intrinsic and extrinsic motivators applied at
the one time is unfortunately the most common situation in the modern
world, a less than an ideal state of affairs. Where it occurs that both
intrinsic and extrinsic motivators are acting in the same direction,
self-determination theory suggests that people tend observe their own
actions and because extrinsic motivators are more noticeable or
salient, they therefore attribute their own actions more to those
motivators than intrinsic motives. This in turn makes people less able
to experience intrinsic pleasure, because they therefore tend to
attribute such pleasure to the extrinsic motivators if they are
pleasurable, or it tends to dull the pleasure if the extrinsic
motivators are painful.
This,
of course, is somewhat tempered by the initial strength of our
intrinsic motivation in the situation. If a person's motivation is
already high, their intrinsic motivation will be reduced, but it will
still be higher than that of someone who was not very intrinsically
motivated in that situation. It will also tend to more readily spring
back into action if the extrinsic motivators are removed.
Our
concept of self also plays a part in this, in that people will want to
continue actions that are consistent with their concept of self. This
may lead them to discount their perception of extrinsic rewards and
punishments as possible motivators if intrinsic motivation is high, and
escalate their perception of intrinsic pleasure, if their intrinsic
motivation is already low.
- Where
both intrinsic and extrinsic motivators are conflicting. This
state of affairs is unfortunately also quite common in the modern
world. Where both intrinsic and extrinsic motivators are acting in
opposite directions, self-determination theory suggests that people may
be in three very different sorts of situations.
Antisocial
influence. One is the sort of situation where people may be
already intrinsically motivated to do something that is socially
acceptable, but some other person for their own reason does not want
them to continue with the activity. The other person offers them a
bribe or threat to try and prevent them continuing with the activity.
In this situation people may be almost forced to perform actions that
are unacceptable to their concept of self, and unacceptable to society.
This causes them to become very conflicted. Their need to integrate
their concept of self with their actions will lead them to likewise
change their beliefs about either the extrinsic motivators being
applied or about the innate pleasure of the activity.
In
order to enable this integration of the self people have four options:
If
they choose to change what they believe about the extrinsic motivators
1/
They may either alter their perception of the offered reward,
discounting the value they place on that reward, or they may alter
their perception of the threat, minimizing their understanding of the
severity of that threat. In this case, intrinsic motivation may
actually improve as the internalized behavior becomes more integrated
and antisocial behavior is avoided.
2/
Or they may alter their perception of the offered reward, exaggerate
the value they place on that reward. Similarly they may alter their
perception of the threat, maximizing their understanding of the
severity of that threat. In this case, intrinsic motivation will
actually diminish as the internalized behavior becomes more integrated
and antisocial activity is implemented.
If
they choose to change what they believe about the intrinsic motivators
3/
If they choose to alter their perception of the intrinsic pleasure in
the innately pleasurable activity, they may do so by discounting the
enjoyment they receive from that
intrinsic pleasure. In this case intrinsic motivation is diminished as
the internalized behavior becomes more integrated and antisocial
activity is implemented.
4/
Or they choose to alter their perception of the intrinsic pleasure of
the innately pleasurable activity, by exaggerating the enjoyment they
receive from that intrinsic pleasure. In this case intrinsic motivation
is increased as the internalized behavior becomes more integrated and
antisocial activity is avoided.
In
most societies there is a fairly good consensus as to what is
acceptable for society. When people are forced or lured outside these
confines of acceptable regulations, you find society breaking up into
dissident groups, who each have their own regulations for those who are
in the group. This in turn can lead to inter-social conflict such as
race riots strikes and other forms of social fracture.
Social
influence. In the second sort of situation people may be
regaled by agents of the social
groups to which we belong such as authority figures like parents,
teachers, employers etc. to change existing behaviors that are
intrinsically pleasurable. They present social regulations to forgo or
delay indulging in activities, which although they are intrinsically
pleasurable, are either unacceptable to family or society or are
inappropriate for family or society at that time or in that place. Much
of what happens in response to this kind of presentation of extrinsic
motivators depends on how this presentation is perceived.
If
the presentation is perceived to be too strong i.e. presented as a
demand replete with contingent punishments or rewards, the effect will
either be to cause the person to resist (do the opposite), or will
effect un-chosen and involuntary compliance. Both of these will result
in no internalization of the regulation.
-
Introjection.
As the perceived strength of the presentation is lowered, that is to
say, as it becomes more an invitation than a demand, and rewards and
threats are not so much implied, the recipient may be able to
internalize the regulation. Regulations can be internalized, even if
regulations are presented with contingent punishments or rewards, but
in this case they will be swallowed whole, so that they are immediately
in serious conflict with the concept of self. Thus they become
introjects that while they are accepted inside the person, remain
outside the self concept, and can only effect the self concept by
pressuring it. That is to say they become
the equivalent of extrinsic motivation, but a pressure that comes from
inside the self instead of from outside sources. These aliens within
the self trying to control it, are much like Freud's idea of a super
ego but far from being a good thing, are a representation of
fragmentation of the wholeness of the person.
-
Identification.
If the perceived strength of the presentation
is lowered still further, that is to say, it is perceived entirely as
an invitation and not as a demand and there are no apparent contingent
punishments or rewards in evidence (that there is in fact autonomy
support), the recipient should be able to internalize the regulation in
the form of an identification. An identification with a regulation is
its acceptance in principle. But there may be elements within it, that
are still in conflict with the concept of self, because of earlier
conflicting regulations that have been already integrated. The
identification is more inside the concept of self than out, but it is
causing fragmentation of the self concept it self.
- Integration.
Sometimes it is possible that the regulations being presented to a
person are in fact compatible with that person's concept of self. In
this case the person should be able to assimilate the new regulation,
assuming its presentation to be autonomy supportive. That is to say the
regulation is internalized and completely integrated with the person's
self concept. In this way the regulation becomes part of what the
person believes him self to be, and will always try to act on. While
this state is not necessarily intrinsic motivation, it has reached the
point of being fully volitional.
Self
generated internalization. In the third sort of situation,
people, because of their need for relatedness, may be actively scanning
the environment for clues, in order to discover the kinds of regulating
behaviors that are required to make them acceptable to various groups
such as family and society. When they discover such regulations they
may then tend to internalize those regulations making them part of
themselves. In this case the external rewards or punishments are not of
great significance, because the person is acting autonomously to
improve their social competence. However, most of the clues or
cues about acceptable behavior that we find in the external world, come
from the actions of other people much of which is presented to us
intentionally. Thus the possibility of our ever being totally
autonomous in assimilating regulations is doubtful. Regulations come to
us for the most part in the form of influence by others. It is just a
matter of whether that influence is felt by us to be controlling or
allowing autonomous choice. Deci and Ryan put it like this:
"For
integration to occur there must be an opportunity for the individual to
freely process and endorse transmitted values and regulations (and to
modify or transform them when necessary). Excessive external pressures,
controls, and evaluations appear to forestall rather than facilitate
this active, constructive process of giving personal meaning and
valence to acquired regulations."
When
intrinsic motivation exists: Moving from a state where regulations or
motivations conflict to states of less conflict. As the
states of interjection and identification especially, are by definition
unstable, there is usually movement within those states in an effort to
reach a more stable state.
-
Moving
from a conflicted state to one of compliance with external regulation.
If the lure of extrinsic reward or the threat of punishment are strong
enough, regardless of whether they oppose an intrinsically motivated
action or whether the seek to ensure the intrinsically motivated
action, the final result may be the loss of intrinsic motivation to
perform the action. This is because of the introduction of extrinsic
motivators into the mix. If the motivators are perceived to be
controlling, they will automatically cause a decrease in intrinsic
motivation. Thus if the extrinsic motivators are strong enough, they
may be able to reduce intrinsic motivation to zero leaving only the
extrinsic motivators resulting in immediate compliance.
Another
way automatic compliance with external demands can come about, is by
the rejection of choice. With choice comes responsibility. With
responsibility comes unwanted outcomes that were foreseeable and inner
conflict because of what we have done. Thus we may start to convince
ourselves not to choose at all, to do what we are told, and let the
responsibility fall on the shoulders of others.
People
who once did things out of love or justice or esteem can end up doing
those same things for money, for comfort or out of fear. Likewise
people who fail to do the best they can, who fail to do what is good
and right, can find that there is no longer any pleasure in doing those
things any more. External demands become instantly translated into
compliance, and the only pleasure is in external rewards.
- Moving
from a conflicted state to an interjected state. If a newly
presented regulation appears to be socially advantageous, the need for
relatedness will tend to be activated to internalize that regulation.
Where both intrinsic and extrinsic motivators are applied, a regulation
that comes from the intrinsic motivation, will in most cases, be already part of the person's
concept of self. In this case, the new regulation will be internalized
by action of the need for relatedness. It will tend to initially enter
the person completely separated from the concept of self, and in
conflict with the concept of self.
If
the extrinsic motivators applied are not so strong or salient that they
will cause intrinsic motivation and self concept to be overcome
completely. They will be internalized as regulations that are in
constant conflict. They may be sort of swallowed whole, without any
effort being made to modify the regulations themselves, the person's
current concept of self, or the perception of the intrinsic and
extrinsic motivators. This causes inner conflict on two levels. First,
it causes inner conflict between the regulations and the intrinsic
pleasure that would result if the regulations were not obeyed. Second,
there is also the possibility of conflict between the person's current
concept of self and the new regulations. This a very unstable and
painful situation.
In
an interjected state the new regulation tends to pressure the person to
act in a way as if he were extrinsically motivated, despite the absence
of extrinsic motivation. But the more he does so the less real
intrinsic motivation he is able to feel. The fact is, that intrinsic
motivation tends to diminish where extrinsic motivators are perceived
to be present. When extrinsic motivators are introduced into the mix
the result will be less intrinsic motivation unless a serious effort is
made to make it appear that the person still has choice. This
unfortunately also happens when the pressure comes from inside the
person. What ends up happening, is that there is say an intrinsic
reward urging a person to do something, but there is also an extrinsic
reward or punishment preventing the person from doing it. The person
trying to tip the balance in favor of the extrinsic reward pressures
himself not to perform the action. But this has the unfortunate side
effect of reducing the intrinsic pleasure even more. If this situation
continues long enough the regulation will become completely extrinsic.
On
the other hand, the need for autonomy will then tend to act to decrease
this conflict by trying to move the regulation toward being more
integrated with the self concept. In this way, the person becomes
apparently more autonomous in that the activity, the action or
inaction, becomes more what the person would choose himself. To put it
another way, the regulation which was originally internalized as an
introjection, which is unstable, was through the action of the need for
self-determination, moved toward being more identified. Two factors
determine which way the new regulation will move. The first factor is
the amount of time the regulation remains in a state of conflict. The
more time it remains there, the more likely it will stabilize in a
state of continual conflict or move toward being completely extrinsic.
The other factor is the amount of autonomy support that is provided,
both with the regulation and generally. This autonomy support will be
crucial in facilitating the new regulation in moving to a more
identified state. The less apparent control, and the more apparent
choice, the more the regulation will be identified
with.
- Moving
from an conflicted state to an identification state. If a
newly presented regulation appears to be socially advantageous, and a
considerable amount of autonomy
support is provided, or rather there appears to be no sign of control,
the need for relatedness will tend to be activated to internalize that
regulation. Where the already internalized intrinsic regulation and the new regulation are
conflicting, as previously stated, the intrinsic regulation will in
most cases be already
part of the person's concept of self and the new regulation will enter
the person outside the concept of self. But when regulations are
internalized totally through the action of the need for relatedness or
with sufficient autonomy support the person will be able to identify
with the new regulation, and bring it inside the concept of self
causing the self concept to be fragmented by having elements within
itself in conflict, such as the regulation derived from intrinsic
pleasure and the new regulation.
When
a new regulation is internalized in this way, there will be
sufficient choice available to the person, that the regulation can be
already modified sufficiently. In this way, it will already be much
closer to the contents of self concept, when it enters into that self
concept. There will still be an inner conflict on two levels but the
important one will be the conflict within the self concept. The need
for autonomy will tend to act to resolve these differences.
In
an identified state the new regulation does not pressure the person to
act as if the new regulation is already part of the concept of self. It
does not need to, as it is already identified with it and is
effectively already a part of it. Consequently, there is far less
likelihood of the person moving the new regulation toward being more
extrinsic and it is much more likely that it will now tend to become
completely integrated into the concept of self. Except in the rare
possibility of psychological problems (like multiple personalities)
this resolution is almost inevitable.
In
this case the need for autonomy may cause the new
regulation to become integrated in four different ways. Firstly, the
new regulation can be modified further, so that it is compatible with
the older elements in the self concept. This rather like Piaget's idea
absorbing new knowledge, where the new regulation is assimilated by
being itself changed. Secondly, the new
regulations may be integrated where the old regulations within the self
concept are modified, changed, or eliminated in so the new regulation
may be integrated. This idea is very much like Piaget's idea of
accommodation, where old knowledge is changed to allow new knowledge to
be absorbed. Thirdly new regulations can be integrated where the amount
of intrinsic pleasure one receives from not following the new
regulation is perceptually changed so that there is little or no
conflict on that level. Fourthly, there is also the possibility that
there may be some intrinsic pleasure in following the new regulation
and that the perception of the intensity of that pleasure may be
manipulated to facilitate
integration.
-
Moving
from a conflicted state to an integrated state. If a newly
presented regulation appears to be socially advantageous, and a
considerable amount of autonomy
support is provided, or rather there appears to be no sign of control,
the need for relatedness will tend to be activated to internalize that
regulation. If, in addition, the amount of intrinsic pleasure to be
derived from following an already integrated and opposing regulation is
very slight, it may be possible for the new regulation to be
internalized and integrated immediately. This would also require that
the new regulation presented no other conflict with the self concept.
Thus the new regulation would become part of a new completely stable
and integrated self concept, rather than a fragmented one. When a new
regulation is internalized in this way it is assimilated with little
change and is accommodated only in so far as there is need to reduce
our perception of the intrinsic value of not doing it to zero.
In
an integrated state the new regulation is already a part of the concept
of self. Consequently there is no likelihood of it drifting back to a
less integrated state. There is however, a possibility that if the new
activity that follows from the activation of the new regulation, is in
itself intrinsically pleasurable that the regulation will eventually
become an unnecessary, and the motivation become entirely intrinsic.
The more the activity in question is performed, the more intrinsic
pleasure the person will receive from that action. Ultimately, the
person will start performing the action because of the intrinsic
pleasure, and the regulation as a social necessity will gradually
become an action or inaction of enjoyment.
-
Moving
from a conflicted state to an intrinsically motivated state. It
is probably never possible to move directly from being motivated
intrinsically to avoid something like sport to a state of being
intrinsically motivated to play sport. If, however, the newly presented
regulation of playing sport appears to be socially advantageous, and a
considerable amount of autonomy
support is provided in the presentation, the need for relatedness will
tend to be activated to internalize that regulation. Normally the
introduction of the new regulation initially produces a conflict
between the older intrinsic motivation and the newer extrinsic
motivation. It is possible though, if a person does not perceive any
attempt to control in the presentation of a new regulation to instantly
absorb the new regulation and completely integrate that new regulation
into one's concept of self as explained above by reducing the value of
not playing sport to zero.
Once
a new regulation is completely integrated with the self concept it is
almost inevitable that the activity required will become intrinsically
motivated, if intrinsic pleasure is innate in that action. In the case
of playing sport (which is innately pleasurable) that pleasure will
ensure that the playing of sport will quickly become completely
intrinsically motivated. Every time a person plays sport the innate
pleasure of playing sport is building into intrinsic motivation and the
need to be motivated by one's sense of self can become less
important.
EXTERNAL DEMANDS FOR ACTIONS THAT ARE INTRINSICALLY
REWARDING CAN BECOME ANY OF FIVE STATES OF REGULATION, EXTERNAL,
INTROJECTED, IDENTIFIED, INTEGRATED OR INTRINSIC.
The
introduction of new regulations where no intrinsic motivation exists.
The
important but unpleasant things we all do. Although a great
many activities can and do involve intrinsic pleasure there are some
activities that just plain do not. They are the actions that, if they
are not done, will in some cases eventually lead to unpleasantness, but
it is not specific that one particular person has to do them. These are
the actions where we would say the person is selfish and lazy if he
does not do them, because if he does not do them, somebody else has to
do them. These are every day things we all have to learn to do as part
of being socialized. They are the things that we do as part of
cooperating with and caring about other people. They include such
things as taking out the garbage, cleaning our rooms and washing the
dishes. There are also a group of actions that must be learned so we
can be considerate of others. These actions include such things as
refraining from talking or making loud noises when someone else is
concentrating or doing delicate work or wishes to sleep or relax. It
means not doing things that are hurtful or unpleasant for others. It
also means learning to do a few things that are culturally specific,
like standing up so an older person can sit down, opening the door for
women, or acknowledging someone of a higher station in life with a sir
or a bow etc.
How
do we learn these unpleasant behaviors? How do they become an
automatic part of our self, that does not even require prompting, when
there is no intrinsic pleasure involved? Behaviorists would say that
the pleasure of extrinsic reward is associated with a behavior and
reinforces that behavior. Social scientists in general are now fairly
certain that this not what happens. It is certainly true that people
will continue to perform an unpleasant or boring action if a reward is
promised or if a punishment is impending if they fail to perform. The
problem is that if the punishment or reward is removed the activity
ceases. Behaviorists tried to get around this problem by using
intermittent reward or intermittent punishment, but although behaviors
lasted longer after intermittent punishment or reward, they still
eventually disappeared. This cannot be the way we learn these behaviors
that stay with us for life.
Humans
are proactive beings. Deci and Ryan's theory proposes that
children are already motivated to absorb these regulations that ensure
human operation in societies involve a minimum of conflict, as part of
the functioning of their the needs for self-determination, competence
and relatedness. In this case, the need for relatedness would be doing
the initial work. Deci and Ryan suggest that the child really only
needs to be brought to the point of performing the action by offering a
minimum of reward or the inducement of a minimum of punishment, if the
required action is not forthcoming by means of social
contagion.
Social
contagion. Social contagion can operate through observing
others in a social context, where they are seemingly being
intrinsically motivated, although they actually are not. When observing
others performing, the typically unpleasant but socially necessary
actions, where no extrinsic motives are discernible, it is possible to
perceive the action as being intrinsically motivated. If those actions
are then performed, they can provide some slight satisfaction in all
three of Deci and Ryan's needs. The need for relatedness is activated
to make the person want to internalize a new way of regulating their
behavior, thus providing satisfaction for the need for relatedness.
Once the person has performed the action, they will have become
socially more competent, satisfying the need for competence. Finally
the need for autonomy or self-determination activates to ensure that
the regulation begins a journey toward becoming a structurally
integrated part of the concept of the person's self. More importantly,
the final resolution of inner conflict that comes from integrating the
new regulation into the self concept and the performing of actions that
come from an unconflicted self concept, may be considered to provide
some measure of satisfaction also. Thus it is possible and preferable
that the internalization of unpleasant, but socially need regulations
can proceed through social contagion just the same as intrinsically
pleasurable actions can.
Social
nutriments. Deci and Ryan also suggest that this
socialization usually comes about through some small amount of
extrinsic inducement. They suggest however, that regardless of whether
the person is being motivated through social contagion or through
external pressure of a minimum of extrinsic motivation, it will require
the contextual saliency of certain social indicators. These social
indicators allow those being motivated to attribute a lack of extrinsic
motives to the person who is applying the extrinsic motivators. They
propose that certain environmental nutriments are needed to be
available to the person who is attempting to internalize the new
regulation. These nutriments have to do with partial satisfaction of
the three principle needs. Obviously the younger these regulations can
be internalized the better. Thus it is important that we have a clear
idea how children internalize these regulations. Deci and Ryan's
research indicates that a child needs to be involved in the process as
much as possible so that he or she takes responsibility for what
happens.
Some
of these nutriments are:
-
Clear
informational limits as to what is required.
-
Clear
understanding of why a new regulation is required by the society,
parent or group that is requiring the regulation. This ensures a sense
of anticipated acceptance, belonging and relatedness.
-
Clear
understanding of how the internalization of the regulation will enable
social competence providing anticipation of that competence.
-
Clear
choice within the limits. Choice is the most important of the
nutriments. This process is facilitated by giving a child as much
choice in the matter of a new regulation as possible. This can be given
not just in the variations of the form of the action and its time or
place, but also choice in what is suitable as a reward if the child
performs, or what punishment will be applied if he does not. This can
take the form of an informal contract between the parent and the child,
as soon as the child is able to understand the concept. Thus the child
becomes as responsible for the outcome as is the parent.
When
intrinsic motivation does not exist: Moving from a state where
regulations or motivations conflict to states of less conflict.
As the states of introjection and identification are by definition
unstable there is usually movement within those states in an effort to
reach a more stable state.
- Moving
from an inactive state to one of compliance with external regulation.
Even though we may perceive certain activities as providing no
intrinsic pleasure, and thus seeing no reason to indulge in those
activities, society may regard those activities as essential. There can
be no doubt that the introduction of rewards or threat to induce
engagement in those activities will work. The question is of course,
"Will people continue to engage in those activities if the rewards or
threats are withdrawn?" The answer is of course that they will not. The
question then we must ask is, "Is there another way of socializing
people?" The answer is that socialization is not something we have to
do for others, but is rather something each child does for
himself.
Why
would a child do something he does not enjoy unless threatened or
bribed? The fact is children do this all the time. They do these things
because these are required for entrance into various groups within
society. In order to belong, they are prepared to do things that are
otherwise distasteful. Children are in fact monitoring the environment
around them all the time looking for social indicators about how they
should act. Given the right kind of environment, such as, most people
modeling a certain way of acting in a situation, most children will act
the same way. The will be quiet in church or in a movie theater.
Indeed, Maria Montessori believed that the reason children often acted
like little barbarians, was because often they did not realize they
were upsetting others, and even if they did, they didn't know how to
perform in a way that did not upset others. Maria was able to help
children to become so respectful of adults and other children, that
they became models for correct behavior.
All
that is required to enable children to become socialized is to provide
an environment of choice and positive feedback about improvement and
accomplishment and models of behavior for the children to imitate, to
produce internalization in the children. Even though there may be no
intrinsic pleasure in an action or inaction, children may internalize
new regulations.
If
this internalization does not occur, this obviously does not mean the
new regulation is not complied with. It may in fact, be readily
complied with, as suggested above, while reward is given or punishment
threatened.
It
is of course not likely, that if such regulations are internalized,
that they would do so by immediately integrating into the person's
concept of self. Especially if the new regulations (the things they are
required to do) are too unpleasant, or too much to do, or interfere
with other activities that do provide intrinsic pleasure, then the
regulations most likely will only be accepted in principle, and taken
into the mind in the form of an introjection.
- Moving
from an inactive state to an introjected state. If a newly
presented regulation appears to be socially advantageous, the need for
relatedness will tend to be activated to internalize that regulation,
despite there being no intrinsic motivation. Where only extrinsic
motivators or no motivators are applied other that the intrinsic desire
to be socially connected, a regulation may be internalized separate
from ones concept of self as an introjection. This is also the case if
other intrinsic or extrinsic motivators are in opposition to this new
regulation. This introjection thus will initially, tend to be in
conflict with the concept of self.
If
the extrinsic motivators applied are not so strong or salient that they
cause the self concept to be overcome completely, new regulations can
be internalized as motivations that are internal but in conflict with
the self concept. These regulations may be sort of swallowed whole
without any effort being made to modify either the regulations
themselves or the person's current concept of self or the perception of
the extrinsic motivators. This causes inner conflict between the new
internalized regulations and the person's understanding of what it is
to be himself. This a very unstable and painful state of disharmony.
In
an introjected state, the new regulation tends to also pressure the
person to act as if he were still being extrinsically motivated, even
after the extrinsic motivators have been withdrawn. But the more the
person pressures himself in this way, the less real intrinsic
motivation he is able to feel. The fact is, that intrinsic motivation
tends to diminish when extrinsic motivators are perceived to be
present. When extrinsic motivators are introduced into the mix, the
result will be less intrinsic motivation, unless a serious effort is
made to make it appear that the person still has choice. This
unfortunately also happens when the pressure comes from inside the
person. The person trying to tip the balance in favor of the new
regulation pressures himself to perform the action, but this has the
unfortunate side effect of reducing the intrinsic pleasure even more.
If this situation continues long enough the regulation will likely
revert to being completely extrinsic.
On
the other hand the need for autonomy will then tend to act to decrease
this conflict by trying to move the regulation toward being more
integrated with the self concept. In this way, the person becomes
apparently more autonomous in that the activity, he becomes more what
the person would choose himself. To put it another way the regulation
which was originally internalized as an introjection, which is
unstable, can be through the action of the need for self-determination,
moved toward being more identified. Two factors determine which way the
new regulation will move. The first factor is the amount of time the
regulation remains in a state of conflict. The more time remains there
the more likely it will stabilize in a state of continual conflict or
move toward being completely extrinsic. The other factor is the amount
of autonomy support that is provided both with the regulation and
generally. This autonomy support will be crucial in facilitating the
new regulation in moving to a more identified state. The less apparent
control, and the more apparent choice, the more the regulation will be
identified with.
- Moving
from an conflicted state to an identification state. If a
newly presented regulation appears to be socially advantageous, and a
considerable amount of autonomy
support is provided, or rather there appears to be little sign of
control, the need for relatedness will tend to be activated to
internalize that regulation, despite there being no intrinsic
motivation. But when a regulation is internalized totally through the
action of the need for relatedness and with sufficient autonomy
support, the person will be able to identify strongly with the new
regulation despite there being no real intrinsic pleasure in the
activity. This will enable them to internalize the regulation
sufficiently to bring it inside their concept of self, causing the self
concept to be fragmented by having elements within itself in conflict.
Old the regulations or old intrinsic motivation may be in conflict with
the new regulation.
When
a new regulation is internalized in this way, there will be
sufficient choice available to the person that, the regulation can be
already modified significantly. Thus the new regulation will already be
much closer to the contents of self concept when it is internalized.
However, there will still be a conflict between one part of the self
concept and another part, dividing the self concept itself. The need
for autonomy will then tend to act to resolve these differences.
In
an identified state, the new regulation does not pressure the person to
act, as the new regulation is already part of the concept of self. It
does not need to, as it is already identified with it and is
effectively already a part of it. Consequently, there is far less
likelihood of the person moving the new regulation toward being more
extrinsic, and it is much more likely that it will now tend to become
completely integrated into the concept of self. Except in the rare
possibility of psychological problems, such as multiple personality,
this resolution is almost inevitable.
In
this case, the need for autonomy may cause the new
regulation to become integrated in three different ways. Firstly the
new regulation can be modified further so that it is compatible with
the older elements in the self concept. This rather like Piaget's idea
of absorbing new knowledge where the new regulation is assimilated by
being itself changed.
The
second way the new
regulation may be integrated is where the old regulations within the
self concept are modified, changed, or eliminated, so the new
regulation may be integrated. This idea is very much like Piaget's idea
of accommodation where old knowledge is changed to allow new knowledge
to be absorbed.
Thirdly
there is the possibility that people may attribute, whatever aroused
feelings they may be experiencing, to being an experience of pleasure.
In other words it may be possible to experience pleasure by simply
changing our perception of what we are experiencing from being
unpleasant to pleasant. While this may be a very difficult kind of
transmutation to accomplish on our own, it may in fact be quite easy in
the presence of role models. There is plenty of research evidence to
show that people who on observing another person and attributing their
performance to the pleasure they are obtaining intrinsically, who then
begin to find intrinsic pleasure in such activity where they did not
before. It has been shown for instance that if a teacher is perceived
as enjoying an activity, even if he is not, this will tend to produce
students that enjoy the activity. When those students then teach they
in turn will produce students that enjoy the activity, and so on. This
more social contagion.
-
Moving
from a conflicted state to an integrated state. If a newly
presented regulation appears to be socially advantageous, and a
considerable amount of autonomy support is provided, or rather there
appears to be no sign of control, the need for relatedness will tend to
be activated to internalize that regulation, despite there being no
intrinsic motivation. If in addition the amount of intrinsic pleasure
to be derived from following an already integrated and opposing
regulation is very slight, it may be possible for the new regulation to
be internalized and integrated immediately. This would also require
that the new regulation presented no other conflict with the self
concept. Thus the new regulation would become part of a new completely
stable and integrated self concept rather than a fragmented one. When a
new regulation is internalized in this way, it is assimilated with
little change and is accommodated only in so far as there is need to
manipulate our own perception of the intrinsic value of not doing it to
zero.
Once
a new regulation is completely integrated with the self concept, it may
be possible that the activity required will become intrinsically
motivated, if we are able to perceive others experiencing intrinsic
pleasure from that activity. When dealing with activities that are
almost defined as being in themselves unpleasant it is difficult to
know how far this can be carried and how far it should be carried.
While it does seem bizarre to think of people experiencing pain as
pleasure, we know masochists do just that, as do people who whip
themselves in religious ceremonies. Consider also the stoics of ancient
Sparta who took pleasure in the hard, simple Spartan life. A better
example of what is meant here, however, is the person who overcomes
great adversity and pain to accomplish something truly amazing. This
somehow seems to be a useful sort of possible intrinsic pleasure
despite the fact that the action is not intrinsically pleasurable in
and of itself.
EXTERNAL DEMANDS FOR ACTIONS THAT ARE NOT
INTRINSICALLY REWARDING CAN BE PROCESSED INTO ANY OF FOUR STATES OF
REGULATION, EXTERNAL, INTROJECTED, IDENTIFIED OR INTEGRATED.
An alternative explanation using the
theory of cognitive dissonance.
Socialization.
This all about socialization. This about how parents and other
socializing factors enable children to be able to do things perform
actions that are acceptable to and encouraged by society. It is about
how they can facilitate change, that allows people to end up doing and
even seemingly enjoy doing things that we would normally consider to be
unpleasant. These agents or parents firstly provide their own actions
as examples of the social norms. Parents etc. however are faced with a
problem. On the one hand they want their children to comply with their
demands for more social behavior. But on the other hand they want their
children to freely choose to perform in this more socialized manner.
This is normal. We must all learn to be toilet trained, when we should
not speak or make a noise, and generally how to be considerate of
others.
The
answer to this dilemma curiously is predicted by Deci and Ryan's theory
to involve only the minimum of extrinsic inducement and that it should
not involve surveillance or any loss of regard. Ideally is should
involve a sort of contract between the parent and the child the
breaking of which automatically incurs the agreed upon punishment or
withdrawal of reward. Deci and Ryan's theory predicts that children are
in fact motivated to find these social norms.
Cognitive
dissonance. There is however, another theory that explains
this same use of a minimum threat or reward for the absorption of the
new regulations into the self. This is Festinger's
"Theory of Cognitive Dissonance". The theory predicts that if
we perform an action that has unwanted consequences that were
foreseeable, the unpleasant feeling of cognitive dissonance will be
aroused, because we have done something unacceptable to our self
concept. The theory also predicts, that we must try to reduce this
feeling of dissonance by changing what we believe about those actions.
This because we cannot change the actions.
It
seems counter intuitive to suppose only a minimum of extrinsic reward
or threat of punishment should be most effective. One would think the
greater the reward or the larger the threat of punishment the less
likely these unwanted behaviors on the part of children would be and
this true. But remember what we want is for the children to choose
freely not to perform these actions in certain places and at certain
times. There have been many experiments in cognitive dissonance
research to also show that a small punishment or a small reward will
always be more effective in causing children to modify their self
concept. It works as follows:
If
a parent demands that a child clean up his room, the parent may also
allow the child to decide when and how to go about it, and he can also
come to an agreement with the child as to what punishment for not
doing, or reward for doing, might be sufficient for the action to be
undertaken. Suppose the child performs the action despite the fact that
the punishment for not doing it was very mild, or he did it despite the
fact the reward for doing it was poor. In this situation, a state of
cognitive dissonance is aroused which the child must somehow reduce. He
has performed the action and cannot change that fact. He can only
change his understanding and perception of the action. The simplest way
for him to deal with this to decide that he had wanted to perform the
action all along. He may decide that he wanted to please his parents or
that it was the right thing to do. He might even decide that it was
enjoyable. Most people would decide it was the right thing to do, but
whatever is decided, the result is the same. It results in the child
perceiving the action as now being less something he does not want to
do, and more something he does want to do. He wanted to clean up his
room he likes to clean up his room.
The
next time the parent wants the child to clean the room the reward or
punishment to induce it may then be lowered still further. Cognitive
dissonance has moved the action of cleaning the room from a place of
being an unpleasant to being one the person wants to do and even
perhaps enjoys.
Modification
of the self concept. Let us take a closer look at how
modification of the self concept takes place: Children may have been
told not to take sweets out of a jar in plain sight. One child is told
that he will be punished severely if he does and the other is told he
will only be punished moderately if he takes a sweet.
Let
us suppose neither child take a sweet. What happens? Well the first
child didn't take the sweet but he still wants to take it. The only
thing holding him back is the large threat. The other child realizes
that the punishment he was threatened with wasn't very much at all. Why
then did he not take the sweet? Here an unwanted but foreseeable
consequence has occurred. The child didn't get the sweet and yet there
was hardly any reason why he shouldn't. Cognitive dissonance has been
aroused. The easiest way for the child to reduce this dissonance is to
modify what he believes about the sweets or about how much he wants to
do what his parents tell him. So maybe his desire for the sweets is
reduced or his desire to do what his parents tell him has increased.
Either way he now finds that he has changed his mind about taking the
sweet and no longer wants to. At this point the parent might reduce the
punishment still further or even eliminate it all together.
Okay
so what happens if both boys take the sweet? Cognitive dissonance
theory provides, that in this second case, the boy who was severely
punished will have cognitive dissonance aroused in his mind. He took
the sweet and was severely punished an unwanted but foreseeable
consequence has occurred. The severe punishment was terrible. How can
he reduce the cognitive dissonance? Well, he could increase his
estimation of the worth of the sweet, or he can decrease his perception
of the severity of the punishment. Either way, he ends up wanting the sweet more than ever, and is more
likely to take it again than he was previously. What about the boy who
is only punished moderately? Well dissonance is not aroused in him. He
may decide the sweet was worth the punishment. He is pretty much in the
state he was in before. His parent may now increase the punishment a
little to try to get him to stop. But it should not be increased much.
Remember after he has been punished once he may not ever want to take a
sweet again. Maybe it wasn't worth it. The answer then is to keep
punishment low. The same is true of rewarding keep it to the minimum.
One
more motivational state.
Six motivational states.
There are six motivational states. Five have already been covered in
detail and they are intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation and the
three in between states of introjection, identification and
integration. There is however a sixth motivational state and that is
the state of not being motivated. The term to describe this state is
amotivation, so that people can be described as being amotivated. You
may wonder why it is necessary to use
this word instead of using unmotivated or non-motivation. The fact is,
these other words describe a whole range of different activities that
are not the same as what is meant by amotivation. If a child resists or
does the opposite of what is required we could say he is unmotivated
but this not amotivation. If a child is lazy and tries to get as much
as possible by doing as little as possible we could say he is
unmotivated, but this is not being amotivated either. Amotivation
simply refers to not being motivated because one does not believe
motivation makes a difference. The early term for this was 'learned
helplessness'.
Amotivation. Amotivation was
conceived as a response to the early experiments by Martin Seligman and
colleagues. In those experiments it was discovered that if humans, or
any creatures for that matter, are placed in an environment where there
is no way of acting to escape a bad or painful situation, then the
creatures or humans learn not to act. The resisting or lazy person is
still motivated, no matter that it is motivation in an nonfunctional
manner. With learned helplessness there is no longer any motivation
because any attempt to do anything is seen as unlikely to succeed. The
person begins to perceive himself as powerless to affect or change the
world around him, and in particular to change himself. In severe cases
we call this depression. The thing is, however, that a person can be
amotivated in some particular area such as academic studies or social
effectiveness without being depressed. Deci and Ryan put it like this:
"Autonomous and controlled activities
involve different types of regulatory processes, yet both are instances
of intentional (i.e., motivated) behavior. In contrast, amotivation is
a state in which people lack the intention to behave, and thus lack
motivation as that term is defined in the cognitive-motivational
tradition. According to SDT, people are likely to be amotivated when
they lack either a sense of efficacy or a sense of control with respect
to a desired outcome—that is, when they are not able to regulate
themselves with respect to a behavior. Amotivation is shown at the far
left end of the continuum in Figure 1. All forms of extrinsic
regulation, even the most controlled, involve intentionality and
motivation, so amotivation stands in contrast to intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation, for it represents the lack of both types of
motivation and thus a complete lack of self-determination with respect
to the target behavior."
A continuum of motivation. We
can see from the above illustration that amotivation is the
form of least intention, least motivation and least self-determination.
Summary
and Integration.
Self-determination theory is concerned primarily
with explaining the psychological processes that promote optimal
functioning and health of human beings. It is about what we learn,
where we learn, when we learn, how we learn and why we learn, for which
the answer is to become optimal functioning and healthy human being. It
assumes human beings to be active, growth-oriented organisms who are
naturally inclined toward the development of an organized coherence in
their psychological makeup and between themselves and the social world.
However, these natural developmental tendencies toward autonomy (i.e.,
internal integration) and homonomy (i.e., social integration), like
other natural tendencies such as intrinsic motivation, are assumed to
require nutriments or supports from the social environment to function
effectively.
More specifically, the natural human mental
self-organization and an organized relation to a larger social
structure are understood to require satisfaction of the three innate or
fundamental psychological needs for competence, autonomy, and
relatedness. Thwarted satisfaction of these needs results invariably in
negative functional consequences for mental health and often for
ongoing persistence and performance. How do we satisfy those needs?
Well autonomy is satisfied primarily by the perception of choice or
options and our determination to take advantage of those choices and
options. Competence is satisfied primarily by informational positive
feedback about what we have accomplished and in response to what we
have been able to accomplish. Relatedness is satisfied through
informational positive feedback we receive in response to the
application of the social skills we have learned. Accordingly, needs
are the linking pin between the gifts and demands of the social world
on one hand, and either people’s natural tendencies toward growth and
well-being or their tendencies toward self-protection with the
accompanying psychological costs on the other hand.
|