What is Neuroplasticity?
A
dark-age of science. For four hundred years it was believed
that brain anatomy was fixed. Norman Doidge M.D. in his book
"The Brain that Changes Itself" puts it like this, "The
common wisdom was that after childhood the brain changed only when it
began the long process of
decline; that when brain
cells failed to develop properly, or were injured, or died, they could
not be replaced. Nor could the brain
ever alter its structure and find a new way to function if part of it
was damaged. The theory of the unchanging brain decreed that people who
were born with brain or mental limitations, or who sustained brain
damage, would be limited or damaged for life." The analogy
used to understand the body during that four hundred years was that of
a machine. Even today people still tend to use that analogy, which
leads to the idea that the brain is the processor, that the brain is
hardware not software and that we speak of aspects of mind or brain as
being hardwired, immutable.
Neurogenesis.
While it was once thought that brain cells
simply died off and no new cells were generated, it is now known that
this is not the complete picture. Through the work of Joseph Altman,
Michael Kaplan, Fernando Nottebohm and Elizabeth Gould, it is now known
that at least one part of the brain, the hippocampus, continues to
receive new cells throughout life, in a process called neurogenesis. It
is now known that in the sub-ventricular part of the brain there is a
reservoir of seed cells (also called stem cells) or undifferentiated
cells. It is now held that these stem cells produce differentiating
cells in this area in response to focused learning or dealing with
totally new phenomena, which cannot be dealt with using previously
learned actions. Where in fact totally new actions have to be learned.
These seed cells divide and then half of them migrate to the area of
the brain that is being used in the new learning. While doing this they
change and differentiate into neurons of the type necessary for
building up the area applied in the new learning. The old theory is
dead. Although neurons cannot divide and grow like other cells in the
body this is misleading. New brain cells could be formed and then could
migrate to whatever area of the brain that they are needed in. This
process of growth or regeneration in the brain is called neurogenesis.
It
was found that novel and challenging environments would in all
creatures including man stimulate the division of these stem cells and
produce the required neurons. This in turn would prolong the average
life of the animal and increase its brain power.
Although
the work that brought neurogenesis to light happened over a period of
time and includes the work of Joseph Altman, Michael Kaplan and
Fernando Nottebohm, the person who brought the understanding of this
concept into scientific acceptance was Elizabeth Gould. Most of her
work has been done on animals specifically in the area of animal brains
called the hippocampus. This area of the brain is understood to be
concerned with memory which of course would require constant new
neurons in forming links with the rest of the brain. The hippocampus is
also the area of the brain where many stem cells in the process of
changing into other cells were detected, although they appeared to have
traveled there from the nearby sub-ventricular zone. All this seemed to
indicate the possibility that any area of the brain might be able to
accept new neurons if they could migrate that far.
In October 1999, a study by
Elizabeth Gould et. al., was published that investigated neurogenesis
in the adult primate neocortex.
Gould
and the researchers reported that in adult macaque monkeys, new neurons
are added to three neocortical association areas that are important in
cognitive function: the prefrontal, inferior temporal and posterior
parietal cortex. No neurons were detected in a fourth area, the striate
cortex, a primary sensory area that processes visual information from
the eyes. The new neurons appeared to originate in the sub-ventricular
zone, where the stem cells that give rise to other cell types are
located, and to migrate through the white matter to the neocortex,
where they extend axons. Because these monkeys are very close
genetically to humans, it is or should be now accepted that this
process also probably goes on in humans. In the conclusions to her
paper Gould had this to say:
"These
results suggest that in the adult macaque brain, new cells originate in
the svz
[sub-ventricular zone] and migrate through the white matter to
certain neocortical regions where they differentiate into mature
neurons. At a short survival time
(2 hours), BrdU-labeled cells were
observed in the svz
[sub-ventricular zone]. At longer survival times (1 to
3 weeks), BrdU-labeled cells that
appeared to be migrating were observed in the
white matter, and those with mature neuronal characteristics
were found in the neocortex. In the adult
rodent, the svz
[sub-ventricular zone] produces
new cells that migrate in the rostral migratory
stream to the
olfactory bulb, where they differentiate into neurons. Our
results suggest that in the adult macaque, the svz
[sub-ventricular zone] is the
source of an additional population of new neurons that migrate
through fiber tracts to neocortical regions."
Childhood, adolescence and
the brain. It is now
fairly well accepted that neurogenesis goes on at a fantastic rate as
soon as we are born and that this gradually slows over time till we
reach adulthood. In her book
"Train Your Mind, Change Your Brain" Sharon Begley explains
it as follows:
"...
two groups of scientists, one at UCLA and one at NIH... Between the
ages of ten and twelve or so, they discovered, the frontal lobes (the
seat of such high level
functions as judgment, emotion regulation, and self-control,
organization, and planning) experience a growth spurt with grey matter
proliferating almost as exuberantly as it did during gestation and
infancy: the volume of grey matter increases noticeably, reflecting the
formation of new connections and branches. And then, in a person's
twenties, there is another reprise of neurological events of early
childhood as unused synapses are eliminated so the networks that remain
are more efficient. Other brain regions also remain under construction
through adolescence. The parietal lobes, which assemble information
that arrives from distant neighborhoods of the brain are works in
progress through the mid-teens. They continue to add grey matter until
age ten (in girls) or twelve (in boys), after which unused synapses are
pruned as they are in early childhood. Similarly, the temporal lobes,
which contain the regions responsible for language as well as emotional
control, pack in grey matter until the age of sixteen and only then
undergo pruning."
In terms of the number of synapses and the number
of dendritic branchings does not begin to look adult until people are
between twenty and twenty five. The very interesting issue
here is that we may be able to achieve some control over this process.
It has been shown that this process can continue throughout the adult
life of human beings, but if, and only if, we are willing to make an effort to learn new and
novel things especially skills that require the use of body parts such
as dance or a sport.
Depression.
Depression, not surprisingly, is very much connected to this phenomenon
of neurogenesis. It has been discovered that when the people or animals
are in a state of depression the hippocampus area in the tends to
shrink and become smaller as few new memory neurons are being
constructed there. It has also been shown that this coincides with a
reduction in the division of stem cells in the sub-ventricular zone.
Likewise, it has been discovered that certain chemicals will increase
the production of stem cells, and that they are the same ones that
relieve depression. For the last 40 years, medical science has operated
on the understanding that depression is caused by a lack of serotonin,
a neurotransmitter that plays a role in just about everything the mind
does, thinks or feels. The theory is appealingly simple: sadness is
simply a shortage of chemical happiness. The typical antidepressant -
like Prozac or Zoloft - works by increasing the brain’s access to
serotonin. If depression is a hunger for neurotransmitter, then these
little pills fill us up.
Unfortunately,
the serotonergic hypothesis is mostly wrong.
After all, within hours of swallowing an antidepressant, the brain is
flushed with excess serotonin. Yet nothing happens; the patient is no
less depressed. Weeks pass drearily by. Finally, after a month or two
of this agony, the torpor begins to lift. But why the delay? If
depression is simply a lack of serotonin, shouldn’t the effect of
antidepressants be immediate? The paradox of the Prozac lag has been
the guiding question of Dr. Ronald Duman’s career. Duman says,
“Even as a graduate student, I was fascinated by how antidepressants
work. I always thought that if I can just figure out their mechanism of
action - and identify why there is this time-delay in their effect -
then I will have had a productive career.”
In
December 2000, Duman’s lab published a paper in the Journal of
Neuroscience demonstrating that antidepressants
increased neurogenesis in the adult rat brain. In fact, the two most
effective treatments they looked at - electroconvulsive therapy and
fluoxetine, the chemical name for Prozac - increased neurogenesis in
the hippocampus by 75% and 50%, respectively. The time delay mentioned
earlier is accounted for by the time it takes for stem cells to divide
and migrate to their destination in the hippocampus in large numbers.
While
these are important advances we must remember that neurogenesis is a
double edged sword, the division of stem cells and their
differentiation into neurons merely allows the brain to change more
easily, but how it changes, depends on what is being learned.
Plasticity. Today the science
of neuroscience has completely overturned the old view that brain cells
do not change and we now understand that the brain far from being
rigidly unchanging, is the most adaptable and changing part of the
body. It has been discovered that the very fact that the brain changes
accounts for learning. Scientists call this new idea brain plasticity
or neuroplasticity. When Neuroscientists talk about plasticity of the
brain they are not talking about polymers, they are talking about the
ability of the brain to respond, adapt, and continually change i.e.
that it is malleable and modifiable.
Norman
Doidge became interested in the plasticity of the brain and began a
series of travels to find out what had been discovered and what it
means. He says, "...I met a scientist who enabled people who
had been blind to see, another who had enabled the deaf to hear; I
spoke with people who had strokes decades before and had been declared
incurable, who were helped to recover with neuroplastic treatments; I
met people whose learning disorders were cured and whose IQs were
raised; I saw evidence that it is possible for eighty-year-olds to
sharpen their memories to function the way they did when they were
fifty-five. I saw people rewire their brains with their thoughts, to
cure previously incurable obsessions and traumas." What he is
saying is that he saw evidence that the brain can change itself, not if
we do nothing, but if we are willing to make an effort. In what follows
we will try to examine what is now known about the brain and how it
learns. The evidence suggests that the positive psychologists like
Martin Seligman were at least partly right and is especially supportive
of the work of Carol Dweck and her work on self theories.
The brain can change itself
by thought and activity. Doidge says, "The idea
that the brain can change its own structure through thought and
activity is, I believe, the most important alteration in our view of
the brain since we first sketched out its basic anatomy and the
workings of its basic component, the neuron."
The
competitive nature of plasticity. Plasticity tells us a lot
about learning and the brain. It turns out that the old saying of, "If
you don't use it you will lose it", is truer of the brain
than it is of an arm or a leg. It turns out that the 'brain maps' of
the functional areas of the brain are not the rigid areas that people
studying the brain previously thought. These areas, it turns out, are
only similar because as human beings we tend to learn the same things
the same way. In other words, the human brain is the way it is, because
humans need to be able to process certain information, and various
areas of the brain are specially adapted to processing various
different types of information. But at the same time any area of the
brain is capable of processing almost any type of information. The
idea, that we use only a small part of the brain, is simply wrong. Any
part of the brain that is not being used, will tend to be taken over
for the processing of other information. It is no accident, that blind
people have better, even remarkable, use of other senses. They have
this, because the other senses tend to take over the brain real-estate
that is normally used for the processing of visual input. Norman Doidge
explains:
"The
competitive nature of plasticity affects us all. There is an endless
war of nerves going on inside each of our brains. If we stop exercising
our mental skills, we do not just forget them: the brain map space for
those skills is turned over to the other skills we practice instead. If
you ever ask yourself, 'How often must I practice French, or guitar, or
math to keep on top of it?' you are asking a question about competitive plasticity. You are
asking how frequently you must practice one activity to make sure its
brain map space is not lost to another." It is important to
note however, that this is practice not in the sense of repetitive
action but of practice in the sense of learning new and unique data and
actions.
Language,
rigidity and plasticity. A second language tends to be much
more difficult to learn than the first. While young children can learn
their first language quite quickly, an adult who tries to learn a
second language will find it difficult, it will take much longer, and
it will never be as good. Norman Doidge explains:
"As
we age, the more we use our native language, the more it comes to
dominate our linguistic map space. Thus it is also because our brains
is plastic - and because plasticity is competitive - that it is so hard
to learn a new language and end the tyranny of the mother tongue."
[In fact when a second language is learned it is normally processed in
a brain area quite different to the linguistic area of the mother
tongue.]
"But why, if this is true, is it easier to learn a second language when
we are young? Is there not competition then too? Not really. If two
languages are learned at the same time, during the critical period,
both get a foothold. Brain scans says [Michael]
Merzenich, show that in a bilingual child all of the sounds of its two
languages share a single large map, a library of sounds from both
languages."
Changes in plasticity.
Plasticity of the brain is greatest when we are young. During the
period of infancy and on up into late teens the brain remains in a very
plastic state, and during this time of high plasticity, there are many
critical periods for learning all kinds of essential human activities.
Learning to see, learning to hear, learning to use our other senses,
learning to move intentionally the way we want are all very early
critical periods. Learning to walk and learning to speak a native
language come much later and learning to read comes much later again.
Learning these things after these critical periods are finished makes
them much more difficult but not impossible, especially if still within
the childhood long period of plasticity.
Even after this period is over it may still be
possible to learn or at least relearn these things. There is a story of
a man who was blind with cataracts over his eyes from early childhood
who was able to learn to see again after the cataracts had been removed
although he eventually went blind again. His story was portrayed in the
movie "At First Sight" staring Val Kilmer. It seems that we can learn
these basic functions in later life but there are so many ifs and buts.
On the other hand, plasticity does seem to decrease with age and with
it the ability to learn and more especially the ability to unlearn.
This process is linked to the dieing of neurons, which is with us while
we are quite young and gradually increases. However, we are now fairly
sure this massive decrease in plasticity is not inevitable.
Brain
plasticity appears to be very much tied to the amount of learning that
we do, and never disappears completely unless we allow it
to. On the other, it appears that one of the reasons many tend
to stop learning once they reach adulthood is because the brain has
become much less plastic. In fact, the plasticity of the
brain falls during the entire period of the teenage years. Despite
this, the human brain remains very plastic for nearly twenty years
which is a staggering period of plasticity if compared with any other
animal. After this period of plasticity, learning new things is much
harder but not impossible as many people go on to learn throughout
their lives.
Unlearning bad habits and
plasticity. Norman
Doidge continues:
"Competitive
plasticity also explains why our bad habits are so difficult to break
or 'unlearn'. Most of us think of the brain as a container and learning
as putting something in it. When we try to break a bad habit, we think
the solution is to put something new in the container. But when we
learn a bad habit, it takes over a brain map, and each time we repeat
it, it claims more control of that map and prevents the use of that
space for 'good' habits." That is why 'unlearning' is often a lot
harder than learning, and why early childhood education is so important
- its best to get it right early before the 'bad habit' gets a
competitive advantage.
Important.
The above information is not only critical for understanding all types
of learning but it is even more important to be clear about exactly
what Doidge means.
In
terms of actions or skills he clearly means that some activity that is
repeated will become more and more fixed in one brain area for its
processing. The more times an action is performed, the more variations
of the action that are performed and the more complex the action is,
the more of a brain area it will occupy. Likewise, the more an action
is performed the more fixed in one area of the brain it will be and the
larger that area will be. For instance if a person was to learn to type
with two fingers it would be more difficult for them to learn to type
with ten fingers than a person who started to learn ten fingers from
the beginning. So what are some of the bad habits Doidge was talking
about?
Well
one bad habit witch affects almost all humans is that of jumping to
conclusions when there is no need. When you are being chased by a tiger
the first solution you come up with will be best because you have no
time to come up with another. However most problems we have, do not
require such instant judgment and we can afford to entertain other
solutions, and to arrive at the best one. There are many bad habits
humans tend to have because of our evolutionary heritage. In present
day conditions these are counter productive. Perhaps the most counter
productive habit humans have is to try and find the easiest way to do things the way
that requires the least effort. In an environment where humans are in
constant danger and are producing enormous effort most of the time,
this may be effective. But with masses of leisure time and most of our
work performed sitting in a chair this is not effective. Also the least
effort does not always produce the best solution, which is the same as
saying that the easiest solution is not the best solution. This does
not dispute Occam's Razor which states that all things being equal the
simplest solution is most likely to be correct. Simplest does not equal
easiest. The most important thing we can learn is that doing stuff, or
taking action, or effort is both pleasurable and promotes our health as
human beings.
Knowledge
and the practical application of these ideas. At first glance
one might be tempted to think that we need to be more careful about
what we teach and only teach what is correct. In terms of this abstract
knowledge Doidge is surely not asking us to teach only things that are
correct, as that would be impossible with information changing as it
does all the time. I believe what is implied in what he says is that
information should be taught as theory (which is what it is) with all
that theory implies. While clearly some information has been verified
in many experiments over many years and is held by the community
(scientific or otherwise) to be accepted, it is nevertheless still only
theory and some part of it, or some analogy we use to understand it may
be disproved at any moment. We should, therefore, commend knowledge
simply as practical and useful tool to use until some better more
accurate theory is produced. In doing this, we are in a sense, saying
to the brain, this mapped area of the brain is not finished, we are
still accepting information in this area and will continue to do so.
In
terms of body parts what Doidge implies is quite simple. If you loose a
limb or say a finger the area devoted to it in the brain will be
invaded by the function of whatever is left nearby. In the case of a
finger the closest other finger will tend to take over that area. In
the case of a poor monkey who had two of his fingers sown together the
areas in the brain devoted to the two fingers grew together into an
undifferentiated whole.
Learning and plasticity.
When we compare the brain activity of primitive peoples with modern day
instruments we find that not only are they are processing different
information but they are processing it in areas of the brain that
people who are technologically sophisticated use to process other
skills. People in highly scientific jobs are processing very different
information (and are processing it differently) in the same areas of
the brain than say ordinary laborers. Likewise peoples of different
cultures process different information differently. It is only because
these groups interbreed that our brain maps end up looking a bit
similar. In her book
"The Creative Brain" Nancy C. Andreasen puts it like this:
"Neuroscience adds a new
dimension: it makes us aware that experiences throughout life change
the brain throughout life. We are literally remaking our brains - who
we are and how we think, with all our actions, reactions, perceptions,
postures, and positions - every minute of the day and every day of the
week and every month and year of our entire lives.
During infancy, childhood,
adolescence, young adulthood, middle age, and late life we
all accumulate a trove of experiences and memories. These shape our
minds and brains, and mightily so. We literally become what we have
seen, heard, smelled, touched, done, read, and remembered. Some of us
have smelled cookies freshly baking and have tuned our brains to be to
feel both soothed and hungry at the sent.
Those
of us that grew up in 'radio days' have different memories and probably
different auditory and imaging skills, from those of us exposed to the
graphic visual images that flicker across a television screen. Those of
us that grew up doing arithmetic 'in our heads' in the pre-calculator
and pre-computer era may have greater skills at doing various mental
manipulations, yet we watch in awe and even envy as five-year-olds
swiftly navigate their way through icon-driven menus on any one of the
myriad handheld or desktop computerized devices that currently surround
us. Differences in the environment to which their brains have been
exposed have produced very different brains from a those of a
sixty-year-old."
Culture and the brain.
Norman Doidge in his book
"The Brain that Changes Itself" points out much the same
thing, except to make clear that all these activities mental or
physical that affect the development of the brain are not just a
consequence of the environment, but are the work of culture. He says:
"Neuroplastic
research has shown us that every sustained activity ever mapped -
including physical activities, sensory activities, learning, thinking
and imagining - changes the brain as well as the mind. Cultural ideas
and activities are no exception. Our brains are modified by
the cultural activities we do - be they reading, studying music, or
learning new languages. We all have what might be called a culturally
modified brain, and as cultures evolve, they continually lead to new
changes in the brain As Merzenich puts it, 'Our brains are vastly
different in fine detail from the brains of our ancestors...In each
stage of cultural development...the average human had to learn complex
new skills and abilities that all involve massive brain change...Each
one of us can actually learn an incredibly elaborate set of ancestrally
developed skills and abilities in our lifetimes, in a sense generating
a re-creation of this history of cultural evolution via brain
plasticity.' So a neuroplastically informed view of culture and the
brain implies a two way street: the brain and genetics produce culture,
but culture also shapes the brain."
Doidge
points out for instance, that musicians tend to build very large brain
map areas concerned with whatever is involved with the playing of their
instrument. Playing a violin might involve building a large area that
is concerned with movements of the person's right hand. A study of
London taxi drivers reveals that the longer they have been building up
a map of London in their heads the larger their hippocampus tended to
be. The hippocampus is the part of the brain that normally deals with
spatial relationships and memory. Also, we should be aware that changes
in our circumstances can quickly cause the brain to adapt to those
circumstances. For instance people who for the sake of an experiment
wear prism inversion glasses, which turn the world upside down, after a
short while are able to see the world the right way up. The wiring in
the brain changes and flips the information so they see the world the
right way up.
This is all very interesting but the work with brain injured people had
shown that if one area of the brain was damaged another area could be
used to compensate. People were starting to wonder if the brain maps
that had been drawn up were truly wedded to the functions they usually
performed. For instance does the visual experience arise out of
intrinsic properties of the tissue in the visual cortex or is it
instructed by the eyes to become the visual processor?
An eye for an ear.
Helen Neville was one of those asking this question and she decided to
do something about it. "What if," she wondered, "the
kind of input a brain receives matters...and matters as much as the
instructions it receives from the genes? ...What if, instead,
environmental inputs and thus experiences a person has, shape the
development and specialization of the brain's regions and circuits?" In
1983 Neville began a series of experiments on deaf people using fMRI
scans to monitor brain activity in deaf people.
People
deaf from birth or early childhood were compared with people who had
normal hearing. The subjects were told to look straight ahead and were
subjected to flashes of light at the side of their heads. The response
to he flashes proved to be 2 or 3 times as great for deaf
people as for normal people. More interesting, however, was the fact
that, although normal people registered the flashes in their visual
cortex, the deaf people registered the flashes in the area of their
auditory cortex. Further experiment reveled that while color and shape
information was being processed in the visual cortex of the deaf
people, information about location and movement was being diverted to
the auditory cortex for processing, where it was being processed
considerably better than for people with hearing.
Peripheral
vision in deaf people was not only far better, more accurate and
sensitive, but was being processed in the part of the brain that would
normally be used for hearing. Instead of the auditory cortex withering
away through disuse it was being used for something else. It seems the
that in deaf people the brain compensates for the lack of hearing by
tinkering with the circuits that handle particular aspects of vision
namely peripheral vision and object change of place or
motion.
An ear for an eye.
Blind people are supposed in folk-law to have
magically superior abilities with their other senses but science had
found little evidence of this. Helen Neville set off to see if she
could find evidence of this based on what she had learned about the
deaf. It occurred to her that there may be an equivalent of peripheral
vision a kind of peripheral hearing. In an experiment where speakers
had been placed in four different locations it was found that both
sighted and unsighted people could register changes in tone in the
speaker directly in front of them.
But
although all had difficulty registering sound at the periphery or the
side the blind people were considerably better at it. They were faster
at detecting the changes in tone and the brain activity associated with
this was more easily returned to a rested, ready state. In sighted
people the response to peripheral sound was in the auditory cortex as
you would expect. But in blind people, the response occurred in the
visual cortex. The sharper and more directional hearing was being
processed in the part of the blind people's minds that had been thought
to be reserved for processing
vision.
Reading with a finger.
Brail is a code in which any language may be written. Each cipher is
composed of two columns of 3 possible raised dots allowing for 63
possible variations. They are small. Each dot is only 2.29 millimeters
apart. An ordinary person can brush his fingers over brail and
understand only that there are some raised dots there. With help from a
local association for the blind Pascual-Leone found a group of brail
experts willing to volunteer for his research.
Pascual-Leone's
initial finding was that the brain map of the blind readers fingers was
very different to a normal group of people who were also studied. The
thumb and the middle finger got crowded out of their usual place in the
somatosensory cortex. The somatosensory cortex was not, it turned out,
that strongly wedded to how it represents the body. The brail reading
finger (index finger) region in the brail reader's brains was, however,
much expanded to fill the space left by their thumbs and middle
fingers. Their brains had expanded this region in response to the
demands of learning brail.
Norihiro
Sadato a Japanese scientist working in Maryland was endeavoring to
build on Pascual-Leone's findings decided to use PET (Positron Emission
Tomography) to look at the brains of brail readers while they were
reading brail. What he found was most unexpected. He found that the
visual cortex was activated when the brail readers were reading brail.
The finding was confirmed by using fMRI. Then a process was used to
temporally disable the visual cortex in the brail readers to see if it
interfered with their reading. It did. They could still feel the dots
like a normal sighted person but they could no longer understand what
the brail was saying. They had temporally lost the ability to process
the dots into language.
People
who have been blind from birth or early childhood understand brail with
their visual cortex. The neurons that process visual images into sight
in sighted people find a use in reading the raised dots of brail.
Indeed a woman who had been blind from an early age and who had become
proficient at reading brail suffered a stroke in her visual cortex and
could no longer read brail.
Recalling and
language. Amir Amedi started working with plasticity while he
was still a student at Hebrew University in Jerusalem. He conducted a
number of experiments on blind volunteers. When he began to test verbal
memory it produced a surprise. The blind people were asked to read from
a brail document a list of words and their visual cortex lit up as
expected. But when asked to recall as many of the words as they could
their visual cortex's lit up again. This showed that the brain not only
had altered the place for processing sensory input signals but had
reshaped the brain in the processing the sophisticated cognitive
function of recall. Also the memory of the blind people appeared to be
far better than a group of sighted controls who read the same list of
words normally. To make sure that the lit up area was doing the work in
the visual cortex Amedi temporarily knocked out the visual cortex's of
the subjects with magnetic stimulation. The sighted people were able to
perform verbal activities normally but the blind people were no longer
able to perform verbal operations.
The blind artist.
Esref Armagan was blind from birth, color and perspective are
properties he has learned from what others have told him. He never
learned brail but he paints pictures of recognizable objects in vibrant
colors and uses 3 point perspective. He is a professional artist
painting the sort of images one would expect are only possible with
vision.
When
scientists analyzed the fMRI scans of Armagan when he was painting and
drawing they were surprised to find that his visual cortex was lit up.
Not only that, but when he was visualizing something before he went to
work, his visual cortex lit up in a way indistinguishable from how it
would look when normal people were seeing something. In sighted people
visualization produces similar activity in the visual cortex but the
activity is much quieter than when seeing something.
Learning
redefined. With this new neuroscience knowledge we can
redefine learning as the act of eternally recreating the brain. We even
have some idea as to how this learning (knowledge) is stored in the
brain thanks to neuroscience. The current theory is that new knowledge
is initially stored by strengthening existing synapses which allow one
part of the brain to connect to another part of the brain. This is said
to be short term memory. If lots of these connections are involved the
brain activates various chemicals in the brain that cause new
(connections) synapses to form and grow along with other connecting
brain elements to become separate unique storage
bundles.
Thus short term memory becomes long term memory. In her book
"The Creative Brain" Nancy C. Andreasen continues:
"In this particular case,
when the neuron is
stimulated to a sufficient degree to create a memory that needs to be
preserved, a variety of chemical messages are sent to the cell nucleus,
where in turn genes are expressed and send messages back out to the
synapse that say: 'build more synapses and create new synaptic
connections so that you can keep this information for a long time."
Totally new unique memories,
especially those involving new skills, it is thought likely, may
require the formation of totally new neurons, which are known to form
in the hippocampus and connect to various parts of the cortex of the
brain. Memory may even require the formation of new neurons in the
outer neocortical layers as would be consistent with the recent work of
Elizabeth Gould.
Are the learning associationists correct?
The
idea of association is discussed elsewhere in this site and is the
backbone of the behaviorist ideas. The answer to the question at the
beginning of this paragraph is yes and no. Yes, pleasure can be bonded
with almost any activity. But this is only really effective when the
brain is in a special state.
Falling in love and pleasure.
The pleasure centers are part of the brain's reward system. Found in
the part of the limbic system called the septal region, the pleasure
centers do not really provide pleasure when they fire. The
pleasure centers rather enable any sort of external input to be
experienced as pleasure. Drugs like cocaine have the effect of making
any sort of action or sensory input pleasurable because it lowers the
threshold at which our pleasure centers fire. Similarly falling in love
or maybe bonding with a child can cause the threshold to be lowered and
the pleasure centers to fire.
Pleasure,
associations and firing together. When the threshold is
lowered a person enters an enthusiastic and optimistic state, where
anticipation of gaining his desire is high. This state has been shown
to occur in controlled tests when a person takes cocaine, when a person
is becoming manic, and when a person is in love. During this state
people are sensitive to anything that might give pleasure such as
flowers, a thoughtful gesture or fresh air. This is called
'Globalization'. Globalization not only allows us to feel pleasure more
intensely but also makes it harder for us to experience pain or be
unhappy. Globalization also creates an opportunity for us to develop
tastes in what we find attractive. An unattractive pock mark on a girls
face can suddenly become a beauty spot. Doidge explains it as follows,
"Neurons that fire together wire together, and feeling such pleasure in
the presence of this normally unappealing pockmark causes it to get
wired into the brain as a source of delight." This explains
how associations can build but only at times when this threshold has
been lowered in some manner.
Learned
nonuse and the use of plasticity in treating disabilities.
The idea of learned nonuse comes from the work of Edward Taub. Taub
although originally a behaviorist has done most to show scientifically
that action is initiated in the brain and is not just a reflex. This
arose out of an experiment by Sherrington where the sensory nerves in a
monkey's arm was cut before it reached the brain. The monkey who had
this done simply stopped moving their arm. From this it was theorized
that movement must entail sensory input to initialize activity. Later
the behaviorists generalized this to all activity. Initially Taub tried
to duplicate the experiment that originally gave rise to the idea that
all activity is reflexes. The following is from "The Brain that Changes
Itself" by Norman Doidge:
"Taub
working with a neurosurgeon, A. J. Berman, wanted to see if he could replicate
Sherrington's experiment on a number of monkeys, and he expected to get
Sherrington's result. Going a step further than Sherrington he decided
not only to deafferent one of the monkey's arms but to put the monkey's
good arm in a sling to restrain it. It had occurred to Taub that the
monkeys might not be using their deafferented arms because they could
use their good ones more easily. Putting the good one in a sling might
force a monkey to use a deafferented arm to feed itself and move
around. It worked. The monkeys, unable to use their good arms, started
using their deafferented arms."
Taub
realized that his finding had major implications, if the monkeys could
move their arms without feeling. All his teachers were wrong. This
really overthrew the behaviorist ideas and caused Taub some
trepidation, but with some encouragement by a mentor he eventually
arrived at his theory of nonuse. Taub supposed that people were unable
to use parts of their bodies simply because they stopped trying and
were able to continue with their normal activities some other way. Taub
believed the reason movement was so difficult was not just muscle
atrophy, but brain atrophy, where the area of the brain devoted to the
limb was taken over by other functions.
Taub
reasoned, as in his experiment, he might be able to get people who had
lost the use of their limbs to regain use of them. The first thing he
tried was to put the good arm of a stroke victim in a sling as in the
experiment. This worked very well and he was soon getting very good
results with useless arms. He realized that this should be applicable
to any kind failure in the brain if only he could figure out how to
stop the person coping some other way. This therapy is called
constraint-induced therapy or CI.
In
his early work with monkeys Taub had tried behaviorist conditioning and
discovered it was ineffective but found another technique called
'shaping' to be very effective. Shaping differed in that a deafferented
animal would get a reward not only for successfully reaching for food
but making the first, most modest gesture toward it. Doidge continues, "Taub
has discovered a number of training principles:
training
is more effective if the skill closely relates to everyday life;
training
should be done in
increments;
and
work should be concentrated into a short time, a technique Taub calls
'massed practice' which he found more effective than long term but less
frequent training.
Many
of the same principles are used in 'immersion' learning of a foreign
language. How many of us have taken language courses over years and not
learned as much as when we went to the country and immersed ourselves
in the language for a far shorter period of time."
Taub's
work has been effective in dealing with stroke victims who have not
moved their limb for ten and more years, it has been effective in
dealing with children who had suffered from cerebral palsy, and it has
been effective in helping brain injured people.
The use of
plasticity in treating OCD. If we add up figures all day
long, if we count items all day long, we must expect some consequences,
such as the almost automatic counting of any objects you come in
contact with. Not stepping on the cracks in the pavement, which starts
off as a child's game, can often become part of an obsessive
compulsive's disorder, as are things that are religiously drummed into
us like cleanliness. What is neuroscience saying about this? It is
saying that when we do something that involves the new, the novel, and
requires learning the area of the brain allocated to that activity
grows and become more healthy. If however, we use that area of the
brain less the area shrinks. But if we use the area in a repetitive
manner, doing the same things over and over, we tend to create a closed
circuit in the brain which repeats as a kind of reflex. This
repetitiveness can get lose from voluntary control. When these
repetitions occur involuntarily they are called obsessive compulsive.
The work of Jeffrey M. Schwartz was with OCD
(Obsessive Compulsive Disorder). Using knowledge gained from
neuroscience about plasticity Schwartz developed a successful treatment
for Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Standard treatment for OCD was that
of desensitization or cognitive self confrontation and logic.
Schwartz's take was quite different. He first advised sufferers to
recognize that the content of the obsession was irrelevant. If they
were obsessional about germs, he advised them to think, "yes I have a
serious problem" but it is not a problem of germs it is rather an
episode of OCD.
The
second part of the treatment, was for the sufferer, as soon as an OCD
episode started, to focus on something pleasant like listening to music
or working on a hobby or playing a game. The idea was to shift gears,
to stop going through the OCD ritual and do something else instead.
Like everything in the brain he theorized, the more you do it the more
it becomes entrenched. Doidge explains about compulsions. "With
obsessions and compulsions, the more you do it, the more you want to do
it; the less you do it, the less you want to do it....it is not what
you feel while applying the technique that counts, it is what you do."
The other techniques actually induce OCD episodes
in order to deal with them. But this is counter productive as they
increases the number of OCD episodes. The idea with Schwartz's
technique was to lower the number of episodes. This follows from two
key bits of knowledge about plasticity:
"Neurons
that fire together wire together."
"Neurons
that fire apart wire apart."
Doidge
gave a very good bit of advice to a friend who was having trouble
leaving the house because she kept rechecking whether she had turned
things off and locked the door. This is a well known symptom of OCD,
but it is something perhaps most people do to some extent. Here is
Doidge's advice. "...often we check and recheck appliances
without really concentrating. I suggest you check once, and once only,
with the utmost care."
The
plastic paradox. The plastic paradox is considered by Doidge
to be the most important conclusion of his book and it is as follows: The
same neuroplastic properties that allow us to change our brains and
produce flexible behavior can also allow us to produce more ridged ones.
Doidge continues; "All people start out with plastic
potential. Some of us develop into increasingly flexible children and
stay that way throughout their adult lives. For others of us the
spontaneity, creativity and unpredictability of childhood gives way to
a routinized existence that repeats the same behavior and turns us into
rigid caricatures of ourselves. Anything that involves unvaried
repetition - our careers, cultural activities, skills and neuroses -
can lead to rigidity. Indeed, it is because we have a neuroplastic
brain that we can develop these rigid behaviors in the first place. As
Pascual-Leone's metaphor illustrates, neuroplasticity is like pliable
snow on a hill. When we go down the hill on a sled, we can be flexible
because we have the option of taking
different paths through the soft snow each time. But should we choose
the same path the second time or the third time, tracks will start to
develop, and soon we will get stuck in a rut. - our route will now be
quite rigid as neural circuits, once established, tend to become
self-sustaining."
Please
note how this tends to explain the work of Carol Dweck on mindsets. It
is easy to see people who believe that things are unchanging and can't
be changed, are ridged caricatures and people who believe that things
can be changed by effort have flexible brains.
It
is not surprising that our brains have developed in a way that provides
more and more brain area for abilities that we use all the time. It is
also not surprising that we convert brain area not in use to become
available for frequently used functions. However, this enables the
possibility that skills, thoughts, routine etc. can become more and
more entrenched till we can no longer change them, and our ability to
modify our own model of reality becomes atrophied. The thing is,
skills, no matter how good, can always be improved. We need new things
to occupy our minds, we need to continue learning and to think new
thoughts. There is no need for a routine. Doing things the same way all
the time may seem easier, but the brain and the body, for that matter,
are not constructed to be always in harmony. They are constructed to
replace old information with new information, to solve problems, to
change and to grow.
Language learning where
there are learning problems. The work of Michael Meizenich
with the help of Paula Tallal sent research down new avenues looking
for ways of overcoming mental impairments in children. They set up a
company called "Fast Forward" for the purpose of teaching children, and
then adults, skills that could not be helped by restraint of the body
as in Taub's work. They were interested in the learning or relearning
of language. The idea was to set up ways of exercising those areas of
the brain concerned with language function. In the case of dyslexia
they conceived a theory that the problems children with certain words
might be the result of being unable to distinguish certain sounds. Very
short sounds like 'd', 'p', & 'b' they conjectured
might be difficult to distinguish from each other because the brain is
dealing with such a small amount of information for such a short time.
If the brain had not built up structures for processing this when they
were very young it might be very difficult later on.
The
scientific team approached this problem with special computer software
that could slow down just these particular sounds. The phonemes still
sounded like spoken English, but stretched out the duration of 'b'
before 'aaa' for example. To normal people it sounded like someone
shouting under water but for the dyslexic children it was an
opportunity to distinguish a group of new sounds. This they did and
quickly. Once a child had learned to tell the difference between b and
p in the stretched version the software began shortening the sound by a
couple of dozen milliseconds at a time. The software waited for the
children to distinguish between each sound before progressing. The
software continued to do this till the sounds reached normal speed and
length. The results were remarkable. After a period of only twenty to
forty hours of training all the children could distinguish the fast
phonemes. They were no longer dyslexic.
The
brain, learning and volition. This site has always maintained
that real learning can only be truly accomplished if the person is
interested in learning, and so is intentionally active in trying to
learn. Neuroscience has now bought to light an indication that this may
be true in terms of neurogenesis. In other words, neuroscience seems to
indicate that new neurons are formed in the brain only when the
organism is volitionally engaged in learning, and that the so called
learning that is done under duress is not really learning at all.
Fred
'Rusty' Gage, became interested in the role of learning and exercise in
promoting neurogenesis as mentioned earlier and had installed running
wheels in the cages of a sample of his mice.
Gage
then considered the running wheels and the fact that the rats could
enter and leave the wheel at will and wondered what would happen if
rats were forced to exercise. He set about placing some test mice in
wheels where they were prevented from leaving the wheel, and had to run
or be thrown off the back like a rag doll. The outcome was as he
suspected, the rats who where forced to exercise had produced far fewer
new neurons than the rats that had exercised voluntarily.
While
there are other possible explanations of the decrease in neuron
production such as the stress involved in not being able to escape the
wheel, it seems likely that voluntary character of the action could
have been a factor. If we connect this up with the idea that the
voluntary exercise was a form of learning, then there is a possibility,
that only voluntary learning produces easily recalled long term memory,
as would be indicated by new neurons in the hippocampus connecting to
the cortex.
Neural
Migration. The brain seems to go through a massive
reorganization as we get older. Young people seem to perform most
activities and process incoming data with their temporal lobes while
old people who had continued learning throughout life seemed to process
the same functions in the frontal lobes and more so the more they had
continued to learn. Young people also seem to use the different sides
of their brains for different functions while old people seem to use
both sides of the brain for a single function. As one side of the brain
seems to deteriorate the other side seems to compensate for the lack.
The brain seems to restructure itself in response to its own
inefficiencies.
Half
a brain. Just how good the brain is at compensating for its
own inefficiencies was made clear by the remarkable story of Michelle
Mack, who although seeming fairly normal was born with only half a
brain or rather a single hemisphere. Although she had certain
inabilities, she nevertheless managed to learn to speak. She learned to
speak normally quite a bit later than normal children and although she
learned to do almost everything more slowly than normal people she was
still able to become a fully functioning person. Doidge puts it like
this, "Her life is a demonstration that the whole is more
than the sum of its parts and that half a brain does not make for half
a mind."
Although Michelle is unique in being born with a single hemisphere of
brain this idea of near normalcy of children growing up with half a
brain has other examples. By the mid 1980s a radical operation called
hemispherectomy had become the operation of choice for children
suffering with uncontrollable, life-threatening seizures. They found
that, as long as the operation was performed before the child is 4
years old, the child will still learn to talk, read and write. The
worst a child typically suffers from losing half a brain, is some
impairment of peripheral vision and fine motor skills on one side of
the body, the opposite side of the surgery.
Four
methods of compensation. Thanks to the work of Jordan
Graphman, we now have some idea of how the brain manages to compensate
for its own inefficiencies or damage. He was particularly interested in
loss of memory and loss of understanding of words. He theorized that
memory and understanding were subject to the same 'use it or lose it'
rule of the brain. He figured the more we use a word (in different
contexts) the more easily we would be able to access it and recognize
it or understand it. The memory he theorized was probably the firing of
neurons in different parts of the brain that were connected together.
The meaning of a word might be mapped in one sector of the brain while
the visual appearance of letters might be stored in another sector. Its
sound might be mapped to yet another sector. Doidge explains it like
this: "Each sector is bound together in a network, so that
when we encounter a word, we can see it hear it and understand it.
Neurons from each sector have to be activated at the same time -
coactivated - for us to see, hear and understand at once."
Graphman also theorizes that the brain compensates in four different
ways.
-
Map
expansion. The boundaries of brain map
areas for various functions are constantly changing. The more we use
some function the more it grows and encroaches on nearby areas. Minute
by minute our brain area maps are impinging on each other at their
boundaries, becoming larger or smaller depending on the amount of use
they are given.
-
Sensory
reassignment. This is where, if one sense
is blocked, as in a blind person, the area assigned to that perception,
(in this case the visual cortex) is commandeered by the other senses
such as touch. Brain space is never unused or empty, it is always being
used for something. In this way people who have lost one sense or say a
limb will find they have greater control over those remaining.
-
Compensatory
masquerade. This is like the brain's redundancy
system. There is often more than one way for a brain to approach a
task. Some people use visual landmarks to get from place to place,
while others have a good sense of direction. If one of these brain
areas is lost to injury the brain can resort to the other. In this way
a function that is damaged will hardly seem to be missing because
another function is compensating for it so well.
- Mirror
region takeover. To some extent the two hemispheres
of the brain are duplicate areas of function. Unfortunately the brain
needs to be so complex that the different sides of the brain have over
time developed different functions. However, if part of one hemisphere
fails, the mirror region in the opposite hemisphere adapts, taking over
its mental functions as best it can. This is a kind of redundancy
system that has been cooped by evolution to do ever more complex
things. It still works but it can of course cause problems in whatever
that part of the brain was previously being used for.
Plasticity
and life long, learning. The plasticity of the brain
requires us to review how we understand human potential, and brings
into question whether potential exists at all in terms of talent. If
potential exists, it must be able to change over time. The potential of
a new born baby, cannot be the same as the same person when he or she
is a fully grown adult. Our abilities may be the outcome of various
influences that seek to change us. The efforts of our parents, the
efforts of our extended family, the efforts of our teachers, the
efforts of our community the efforts of our society and the efforts of
our culture may all be involved. More importantly we are changed by our
own efforts to change ourselves. Other important factors are the
observation of and interaction with skilled role models. Ultimately, it
can be said, that we are subject to two great forces of change. Our
genes and a random environment make of us what they will, but we also
make ourselves through our own efforts.
This
whole process of lifelong learning, it turns out, has the amazing bonus
of making people mentally healthier. What has become clear is that
people who continue to learn throughout their lives are better
protected against mental decline. The best thing we can do to
keep our brains healthy is to keep using them as much as possible
throughout our lives. We should remain learners throughout our lives,
continue to try and solve problems throughout our lives, and we should
immerse ourselves in new and novel sensory experiences. If we do all
this there will be many benefits. We will live longer, our brains will
remain healthier, and we can remain more like we were in our youth,
than was ever thought possible. This is not easy to do, but that it can
become easy, if we learn to love the experience of the new and novel
and love learning. If children can be exposed to an optimum learning
environment they may all live full productive, important and great
lives. If we as a species are willing to provide our children with such
an optimum environment, we may be able to solve the problems that have
plagued us since our beginnings, the problems of our genetic
inheritance. If we so wish, we may be able to change ourselves into
something better. By making our brains better we can make ourselves
better.
Science,
is as yet not certain, why all this seems to be the case, but it is
likely that the effort of learning new actions and memorizing material
may trigger neurogenesis, or assist the brain in compensating (through
neural migration) for its own breakdown or inefficiencies.
|