Sleep
and learning. 
It is strange to consider
that sleep may be in some way essential for
learning
because sleep appears, to the normal eye, to be one of the few times
when a person clearly could not be learning anything. Other times when
learning seems impossible is when a person is unconscious, in a coma
or dead. However, it seems that there is a great deal of evidence that
sleep has to have evolutionary advantages and that most of those
advantages turn out to be essential to all kinds of learning.

“It
is a common experience that a problem difficult at night is resolved in
the morning after the committee of sleep has worked on it.”
John Steinbeck
“Even
a soul submerged in sleep is hard at work and helps make something of
the world.” Heraclitus
Does sleep provide an
evolutionary
advantage?
The
brain is in a
constant state of tension
between cells and chemicals that try to put you to sleep, and cells and
chemicals that try to keep you awake. The longer you sleep the more
likely these cells and chemicals will swing toward making you wake up.
On the other hand the longer you stay awake the more these cells and
chemicals will get stronger in trying to put you to sleep.
From
an evolutionary point of view sleep seems to have so many disadvantages
that it seems unlikely that any advantage it may provide could be so
great. In his book
"Brain Rules" John Medina puts it like this:
"Sleep makes us exquisitely vulnerable to
predators. Indeed, deliberately going off to dreamland unprotected in
the middle of a bunch of hostile hunters (such as leopards, our
evolutionary roommates in eastern Africa) seems like a behavior dreamed
up by our worst enemies. There must be something terribly important we
need to accomplish during sleep if we are willing to take such risks in
order to get it. Exactly what is so darned important?"

Lack of sleep is a severe
disadvantage. When
considering how sleep might be an evolutionary advantage we should all
be
aware that lack of sleep impairs all
kinds of human abilities. Loss of sleep hurts attention, executive
function, working memory, mood, quantitative skills, logical reasoning,
and even motor dexterity. Sleep loss or sleep deprivation causes every
part of our bodies and minds to falter and degrades all abilities and
bodily functions. In his book
"Why We Sleep" Matthew Walker puts it
like this: "[These
are] ...the
many and varied ways in which insufficient sleep proves ruinous to all
major physiological systems of the human body: cardiovascular,
metabolic, immune, reproductive."

THE ADVANTAGE OF THREAT
SIMULATION THEORY.
Antti
Revonsuo devised a Theory that would make sleep a very necessary part
of our bodily survival strategies and a very strong and necessary brain
mechanism that would make complete evolutionary sense. The following is
an excerpt from Wicapedia:
Revonsuo's
threat simulation theory claims that much or all
of dream experience is "specialized in the simulation of threatening
events", for the evolutionary purpose of rehearsing fight or flight
situations to better prepare for such instances in waking life (similar
to a
"fire drill"). According to Revonsuo, empirical research supports
this theory by showing the recurrence of threatening situations in
dreaming: of
all of the emotions experienced in dreaming, "fear [is] the most common
and anger the next most common".
An
otherwise supportive 2009 review of threat simulation
theory stated that "The main weakness of the theory is that there is no
direct evidence of the effect of dream rehearsal (or the lack of it) on
performance or on survival rates across generations of ancestral
humans",
and also notes it is not clear why some threat simulations end without
a
reaction from the dreamer. The review finds that "Overall, the
available
new evidence and the new direct tests of the predictions of (threat
simulation
theory) yield strong support for the theory. A mass of evidence
indicates that
threat simulation is a function of dreaming, an evolved psychological
adaptation selected for during the evolutionary history of our species.
On
current evidence, the strengths of the theory seem to outweigh its
weaknesses."
According
to a 2017 study in Sleep, an analysis of the
statistical content of intelligible sleep-talking found that 24 percent
contained negative content, 22 percent had "nasty" language, about 10
percent contained a variation of the word "no", and 10 percent
contained profanity. 2.5 percent of the intelligible words were a
variation of
the word "fuck", which comprised only 0.003 percent of spoken words
when awake. The study authors judged the findings as being consistent
with
threat simulation theory.
Cross-cultural
surveys find that the most typical dream
theme is that of being chased or attacked. Other common negative themes
include
falling, drowning, being lost, being trapped, being naked or otherwise
inappropriately dressed in public, being accidentally
injured/ill/dying, being
in a human-made or natural disaster, poor performance (such as
difficulty
taking a test), and having trouble with transportation. Some themes are
positive, such as sex, flying, or finding money, but these are less
common than
dreaming about threats.
Revonsuo
outlines six “empirically testable” propositions
(Revonsuo, 2000) to illustrate his "threat simulation" theory.

Proposition
1 Dream
experience embodies an “organized and selective
simulation of the perceptual world.” Sensory modalities are fully
integrated
into perceptual dream experience, and the “active dream self” has a
body image
similar to that of the waking self in the “visuo-spatial world.” Dreams
are
composed of interactions that mimic archetypical wake-state experiences
and
situations with people and objects. Revonsuo states that dreams are the
result
of an “active and organized process rather than a passive by-product of
disorganized activation.” The predicable organization of dreams renders
them as
more than “random noise”; rather, he views their function as a
“selective
simulation of the world.”

Proposition
2 Representations
of daily life experiences are absent while dreaming.
Dreamers experience a “selective simulation of the world” biased
towards
threatening situations. The high proportion of negative emotions
experienced
while dreaming correlates to the need for “adaptive responses that
increase the
ability to respond appropriately in adaptively important situations”
(Revonsuo,
2000). The misfortunes and aggression experienced in a dream state may
act as a
simulation that prepares the dreamer in the case that a similar
situation may
occur in the awake state. Evidence lies in the activation during REM
sleep
across regions that are necessary for the production of these
emotionally
charged experiences.
Proposition 3 Real
waking life experiences that are traumatic to the
individual causes the dream-production system to create dream content
that
simulates responses to threats as a mechanism to "mark situations
critical
for physical survival and reproductive success." Revonsuo writes that
"what from a psychological point of view is a 'traumatic experience'
is,
from a biological point of view, an instance of threat perception and
threat-avoidance behavior."

Proposition 4 The
threatening dream content, while not an accurate
depiction of one's real-time experience, seems incredibly realistic and
is
therefore effective and productive practice for threat-avoidance
responses. He
postulates that there is proof of this proposition in that dreamed
action is
consistent with real motor behavior and that "dreaming about an action
is
an identical process for cortical motor areas as actually carrying out
the same
action."

Proposition 5 The
perceptual and motor skills simulated in dreams will
increase the efficiency of an individual's performance of those skills
even if
the dreams are not explicitly remembered. Studies have shown that the
implicit
learning of skills that are important for human performance can be
learned and
actualized without any conscious memory of having learned them.
Furthermore,
Revonsuo writes that "REM sleep physiology appears to selectively
support
implicit, procedural learning."

Proposition 6 The threat-simulation system
"was selected for during
our evolutionary history," implying that it was not innate but rather
came
to be in response to the multitude of threats experienced by human
ancestral
populations. These populations lived in a "more or less constant
post-traumatic state" and the dreaming brain constructed the
threat-simulation technique as an evolutionary tool, resulting in
improved
threat-avoidance skills and, thus, a higher probability of survival.
 
WHY ELSE MIGHT SLEEP PROVIDE AN
EVOLUTIONARY ADVANTAGE?
Almost
all diseases and aberrant bodily functions are improved by sufficient
sleep. With sufficient sleep there is less chance of cardiac arrests
and less chance of any heart problem such as atherosclerosis. Normal
sleep greatly lowers the chance of high blood pressure. With enough
sleep there is far less chance of stroke and kidney failure. Lack of
sleep effects both blood sugar and insulin levels causing them to be
unregulated or irregularly regulated leading to such diseases as the
various form of diabetes.
Obviously
then, lack of sleep leads
automatically to much greater weight gain and eventual obesity. Lack of
sleep has also been found to correlate significantly with infertility.
Sleep deprivation also correlates significantly with all disease and
virus risk especially with all the common ones we all catch such as
flus
and the common cold. Also sleep deprivation also seems to increase the
chances of all forms of cancer. Lastly and perhaps most importantly
sleep deprivation seems to greatly shorten life expectancy.

Matthew Walker in his book
"Why we sleep" explains that sleep is
more than a pillar of good health. He says:
"Sleep
is more than a pillar; it is the foundation on which the other health
bastions sit. Take away the bedrock of sleep, or weaken it just a
little and careful eating or physical exercise become less than
effective..."

After
much scientific investigation over many years the most likely
explanation of the main importance of sleep is not that it provides
rest for
the brain, which is amazingly active during most of our sleep cycle,
but rather that it is somehow essential to learning. It is initially
rather counter intuitive to be told that sleep is essential for
learning, but there is some strong circumstantial and practical
evidence.
Thus
we should not be surprised to learn that
lack of sleep impairs the ability to learn, which it indeed does.
Studies show that lack of sleep impairs cognitive skill. The loss of a
single night's sleep can cause a 30 percent loss in overall cognitive
skill, with a subsequent drop in performance. If this loss of sleep is
increased to two nights and impairment of 60 percent of cognitive skill
can result. But the question is what is all that brain activity that
happens while we sleep? What are our brains doing that is so important?
John Medina tells a funny story that provides a clue:
"Consider
the following true story of a successfully married, incredibly
detail-oriented accountant. Even though dead asleep, he regularly gives
financial reports to his wife all night long. Many of these reports
come from the day's activities. (Incidentally, if his wife wakes him up
- which is often, because his financial broadcasts are loud - the
accountant becomes amorous and wants to have sex.) Are we all
organizing our previous experiences while we sleep? Could this not also
explain all the other data we have been discussing, but also finally
give us a reason why we sleep?"
NREM SLEEP AND THE RAT BRAIN.
John
Medina then goes on to explain about what happened to a rat that had
been learning how to navigate mazes with 500 electrodes implanted in
its brain. The rat fell asleep. This provided a unique opportunity for
researchers to discover what goes on in a rat brain when it goes to
sleep after just learning a new maze route. What they recorded was a
pattern very similar to the pattern they recorded while the rat was
awake and running through the maze. John Medina tells the story:
"When
the rat goes to sleep, it begins to replay the maze pattern sequence.
The animal's brain replays what it learned while it slumbers,
reminiscent of our accountant. Always executing the pattern in a
specific stage of sleep, the rat repeats it over and over again - and
much faster than during the day. The rate is so furious, the sequence
is replayed thousands of times. If a nasty graduate student decides to
wake up the rat during this stage, called slow wave sleep, something
equally extraordinary is observed. The rat has trouble remembering the
maze the next day. Quite literally, the rat seems to be consolidating
the day's learning the night after that learning occurred, and an
interruption of that sleep disrupts the learning cycle.

This
naturally caused researchers to ask whether the same was true for
humans. The answer? Not only do we do such processing, but we appear to
do it in a far more complex fashion. Like the rat, humans appear to
replay certain daily learning experiences at night, during the
slow-wave phase. But unlike the rat, more emotionally charged memories
appear to replay at a different stage of the sleep cycle. These
findings represent a bombshell of an idea. Is it possible that the
reason we need to sleep is simply to shut off the exterior world for a
while, allowing us to divert more attentional resources to our
cognitive interiors? Is it possible that the reason we need to sleep is
so that we can learn?"
It
is now fairly well confirmed that
replaying or recalling of memories of the previous day occurs during
the slow wave
period of sleep and that we experience them as a type of occurrence
that could be classified as a dream. Although the scientific evidence
for consolidation of memories is fairly recent the idea of sleep or
dreaming being integral to memory consolidation is not new. The
earliest known human record of such claims was in the writings of the
Roman rhetorician Quintillian (AD 35-100), who stated:
"It
is a curious fact, of which the reason is not obvious, that the
interval of
a single night will greatly increase the strength of the memory...
Whatever the cause, things that could not be recalled on the spot are
easily coordinated the next day, and time itself, which is generally
accounted one of the causes of forgetfulness, actually serves to
strengthen the memory."
People
vary in how much sleep they need and when they prefer to get it, but
the biological drive for an afternoon nap is universal and it is now
well accepted that it can improve
learning and all brain functions quite a bit.
This
sounds all well and good but unfortunately brain damaged people who
lack the ability to sleep in the slow-wave phase nonetheless have
normal and even improved memories as do people who have REM sleep
suppressed by antidepressant medication. It may well be that while
sleeping and dreaming may not be absolutely essential for memory it can
still be possible that they are important for memory. Our bodies may
simply be able to find other ways of reinforcing memories. We may have
redundancy systems or be able to form new ways of consolidating
memories
when normal ones are not working.

Dreams
or dream like states. If sleep is when our
brains are busy doing something that is essential to our survival then
dreams are where these evolutionary advantages must take place.
Everybody is familiar with the idea that dreams happen during REM
sleep. However, dream like occurrences actually seem to happen in all
stages of sleep. Simply put different types of dream like states seem
to happen at different stages of sleep. There are two main types of
sleep REM sleep
and NREM sleep or slow wave sleep. It seems that during NREM sleep we
have experiences that could be called dreams but which are very
different from most of the dreams we tend to remember. For a start they
tend to be very short and they seem to consist mostly of thinking about
and
the recalling of
experiences from the previous waking period and connecting them
together.
NREM SLEEP
Slow
wave Non Rapid Eye Movement
dreams.
Sleep research has discovered that during slow
wave sleep we have
dream like states. However, whether we dream or not in NREM
sleep may
depend on our understanding of what dreaming is. We are familiar with
dreams from REM sleep because we often remember them after waking
during one. They seem to be a full sensory experience as we might
experience when awake, and for the most part we cannot tell the
difference between REM dreams and waking experience while we are
dreaming.
Rehearsal in NREM sleep, is it
replay or recall?
However, NREM dreams
(for
the most part) seem
quite
different. NREM dreams, as discovered by integrating subjects who have
been awakened from the NREM dream state, seem to be more like
thinking than experiencing. Thinking of course often involves recall,
and this recall (for the most part) is not like actually experiencing
the event in question. The replaying of
memories that were
imprinted from events that occurred the previous day may not actually
occur during NREM sleep. Instead we appear to be simply recalling these
events
in
much the same way we do while awake. However, looking back to our mice
it seems that some form of rehearsal that consolidates memories is
going
on despite the fact that full replay is unlikely.
If
experiences were replayed just as they
were experienced initially the person dreaming would be thrashing about
trying to repeat the actions involved in these experiences. This does
not happen however. We know that during REM sleep action schemas are
activated and experienced but that during REM sleep we are
paralyzed so we do not thrash about. However during slow wave sleep we
are not paralyzed so these action schemas cannot be active. This seems
to indicate that if memories are replayed during slow wave sleep then
the activity schemas are not replayed as part of these
memories. It seems we may have very little actual experience
in these dream like NREM states
and therefore very little memory of such experiences if we are awakened
while in NREM sleep.
Of
course there are aberrant states which normally take place in
slow wave or NREM part of the sleep cycle when humans do
thrash about. These are variously known as sleep walking and sleep
talking. As
most humans do not experience sleep walking and talking they must be
considered aberrations and not part of some evolutionary
advantage. On
the other hand reliving memories of previous experiences while awake
does seem to occur sometimes and when they happen the memory is greatly
consolidated. So there is a remote possibility that something similar
sometimes occurs during NREM sleep.
In
her book "The
Secret
World of Sleep"
Penelope Lewis points out that that some of the evidence that has been
gathered about sleepwalkers actually supports the idea of rehearsal
while in NREM sleep. She says:
"A recent study of sleepwalking
patients illustrated this [rehearsal] by
showing that sleepwalkers, who are much freer in their movements during
sleep than other people, often reenact things they did right before
sleep, providing direct evidence for the off-line replay
[rehearsal] of newly
acquired information."
The two functions of NREM dreams.
The
evidence
gathered about these dream like occurrences and process seem to point
toward there being two
possible functions for slow wave (NREM) dream like states. One possible
function seems to be the strengthening
(reinforcing) of memories (by recalling the memory in our NREM
sleep). At the same
time
during this
phase of sleep a pruning of synapses takes place. So it is likely that
the memories of the previous day that are not recalled (or replayed),
or at least
parts of them, are simply deleted.
Thus
during this period of sleep
memories could be said to accessed by the brain and divided into two
groups important memories and unimportant memories. The important
memories are then recalled while we sleep and the unimportant memories
are
deleted. Of course it is more likely that this process does not occur
over a single night. It might take several nights for the
various synapses in a memory to shrivel up. The memory may not
be
deleted instantly but rather loose more and more potential to be
recovered over several nights until it finally looses any potential to
be recalled. Other
synapses than the ones from the previous day may also be pruned away
during this process.
Sorting out which memories to
delete and which to consolidate?
How
do our brains decide which memories should be suppressed or discarded
and which should be consolidated? It turns out that there are several
ways in which our brains seem to do this. We can make a conscious
decision that a particular memory is important and your brain will take
that into account while you sleep. Someone like your boss or your
teacher may indicate that particular memories are important and you
brain will take that into account also. However, the main way our
brains decide this has to do with attention and focus and how they work
to ensure our survival. It turns out that strong emotions can tag
memories as important and thus likely to be consolidated while weak
emotions tag memories as unimportant and unlikely to be consolidated.
But there is a wrinkle to this. Strong emotions like fear hate and
anger, in particular, also focus attention to such an extent that they
suppress or ignore many of the mundane elements of an experience and
thus streamline the memory of that experience.
The Weapon focus effect.
This
is referred to as 'The Weapon Focus Effect' and is related to time
dilation. It has to do with the allocation of resources used in mental
processing. When we perceive that we are in danger and flight or fight
is activated our brains tend to focus narrowly in on a small set of
incoming sensory data and simply not process the rest of the incoming
sensory data. (We focus on a weapon to the exclusion of anything else.)
Because the brain is thus using far less of its processing resources it
is able to process the small amount of data very fast. In other words
our thinking speeds up and time seems to slow down. This obviously
provides an evolutionary advantage where fast thinking and movement may
save our lives. In this way strong emotions both make memories more
likely to be consolidated but also can eliminate important elements of
the experience. Conversely less strong emotions can thus be more
inclusive while also being less likely to be consolidated.
Sleep
seems to double down on this. Penelope Lewis in her book "The Secret
World of Sleep" explains it as follows:
"After
people sleep they not only remember the car crash better, they are also
less likely to recognize the background street. It looks as though
sleep while working to cement memories that are really important, may
also need to pare down the less important ones, clearing space and
resources for what matters."
Reconsolidation.
So
why should replay or recall of memories tend to consolidate those
memories? It does and it does not. When we recall a memory it is now believed that the memory becomes
flexible and labile. In
other words when we recall the memory from storage it becomes possible
to change it or weaken it or delete it. If we then store away the
memory again we are not so much consolidating it but
rather reconsolidating it. This is important because we do not
always want to consolidate a memory. Sometimes the memory we had previously learned was simply wrong and needs to be deleted. Sometimes a memory in storage turns out to be wrong and there is a better memory available to overwrite it and replace it. Sometimes
the memory simply needs to be updated. It is not so much wrong as
incomplete and new elements need to be added to it. I seems that by
becoming labile a memory becomes so changeable that it can be deleted,
overwritten or updated and not just consolidated. Recall while we sleep
would work the same way but to greater extent. Some synapses are pruned
away during NREM sleep leading to deleted memories. Some synapses are
also strengthened during NREM sleep which could explain both
overwriting and updating. It is well established that when a neuron is activated myelin builds up on
its axon which makes the connection both stronger and quicker which
would explain how recalling a memory might lead to it becoming more permanent. However, there may well be
another way in which a memory might be both strengthened and changed. Maybe
past memories also become
labile
and flexible when new memories are recalled, if the new memory
contains
elements also occurring in an old memory. Those parts of
the new memory that also existed in an old memory would thus
be
further consolidated by being activated again. Or possibly only those
elements
occurring
in both the old memories and the new may be recalled at all. The
memories may have some elements strengthened while other elements in
them are not strengthened. Or perhaps only those elements common to to
both the
old memory and the new memory may be recalled at all. It is also
possible that synapses are striped away leaving
only those elements that were common to both old and new
memories.
Information Overlap to Abstract.
Penelope
Lewis and Simon Durant developed a model as a possible explanation of
how replaying memories during slow wave sleep, in combination with
downscaling of synapses could function in unison called 'Information
Overlap to Abstract'. In her book
"The Secret
World of Sleep" Lewis presents it as follows:
"If
more than one memory is replayed at the same time, the neurons
associated with areas of shared replay or "overlap," will be more
strongly activated than the other neurons... This means, for instance,
if you replay memories of two or three different birthday partys, all
of which involved cake, presents and balloons, but each of which was
held at a different place and with a different set of guests, then
responses in the neurons which code for cake, presents, and balloons
will be stronger than responses associated with locations of individual
parties or the people who attended them. Furthermore, based on the
general principle that neurons which fire together wire together...,
the
linkages between neural representations of cake, presents and balloons
will also become stronger than other linkages associated with these
memories, such as between a specific birthday and her presents or other
people who were at her party... All this strengthening is important
because it means that when synapses are subsequently downscaled these
representations of overlap may be the only thing that is retained. In
fact, as multiple memories are replayed across a night, the more often
a specific representation (say a birthday cake) or pair of
representations (say, both cake and presents) is triggered, the more
likely that specific aspect of a memory is to be retained."
This
process above may well be an
explanation for the formation of concepts. Concepts would be formed by
the elimination of information in memory that is not specific to all
iterations of the
concept. In the case above it would gradually form the concept of a
birthday party. It also would as Penelope Lewis believes explain the
formation of new semantic knowledge. She continues:
"The
realization that birthday parties are associated with cake, presents,
and balloons is an example of the development of new semantic
knowledge. Semantic knowledge is the general knowledge about the world
and how things in it relate to each other. Classic examples of semantic
knowledge are the knowledge that the sky is blue, that Paris is the
capital
of France, and for that matter that birthday parties normally include
cake, presents, and balloons."
Slow waves and electric potential.
During
this
phase of sleep what we measure as slow waves is actually slow increases
and decreases in potential occurring throughout the brain. It is likely
that these slow changes ensure that complete memory patterns fire and
not just bits of those patterns. This rise and fall of potential may
also explain how memories are chosen to be recalled during NREM sleep.
Memories that have
already been rehearsed or replayed while we were awake would require
less potential to be activated while we are asleep. Likewise memories
that have greater emotion attached would also require less potential to
be activated. Thus the potential may simply never get high enough to
activate some memories.
In
his book
"The Chemistry of Conscious States" J. Allen Hobson calls the
above type of possible dream function
'consolidation' as in consolidating memories and consolidating
knowledge
out of data or information.
Experimental
findings about NREM sleep.
In
his book
"Why We Sleep"
Matthew Walker ran a number of experiments that relate learning to
NREM sleep. He explains that during NREM sleep the brain is still
active. NREM sleep seems to function to enhance memory of both facts
and actions. Much has been discovered about this in sleep studies.

FACT
LEARNING ENHANCEMENT DURING NREM SLEEP.
The
function of sleeping before
learning.
In
his book
"Why We Sleep"
Matthew Walker indicates there may be many
things happening during slow wave or NREM sleep that both improve
individual memories and also generally improve our ability to remember
things we
have experienced or recall bits of information we have previously been
exposed to. Walker ran many experiments over the years many based on
the his belief that the hippocampus is the site of short term memory
and that the cortex is the site of long term memory. Now while this
site does not hold that it is likely that memories are kept in storage
areas there is no doubt that these two areas are very significantly
concerned with memory. Regardless, Walker's experiments are well
conducted and of obviously great importance. His first
significant finding in the area sleep's effect on memory was dealing
with the possibility of short
term memory being a finite storage that might be improved by being
emptied during sleep with memories being transfered to a less finite
storage of long term memory. This experiment is reported in his book
"Why We Sleep" as follows:
"How
then does the brain deal with this memory capacity challenge? Some
years ago my research team wondered if sleep helped solve this storage
problem by way of a file-transfer mechanism. We examined whether sleep
shifted recently acquired memories to a more permanent long term
storage location in the brain, thereby freeing up our short term memory
stores so that we awake with a refreshed ability for new learning.
We
began testing this theory using daytime naps. We recruited a group of
healthy young adults and randomly divided them into a nap group and a
no-nap group. At noon all the participants underwent a rigorous session
of learning (one hundred face-name pairs) intended to tax the
hippocampus, their short term memory storage site. As expected both
groups performed at comparable levels. Soon after the nap group took a
ninety-minute siesta in the sleep laboratory with electrodes placed on
their heads to measure sleep. The no-nap stayed awake in the laboratory
and performed menial activities, such as such as browsing the Internet
or played board games. Later that day, at 6 p.m., all participants
performed another round of of intensive learning where they tried to
cram yet another set of new facts into their short term storage
reservoirs (another one hundred face name pairs). Our question was
simple: Does the learning capacity of the human brain decline
with
continued time awake across the day and if so can sleep reverse this
situation effect and thus restore learning ability?
Those who were awake throughout the day became
progressively worse at learning even though their ability to
concentrate
remained stable (determined by separate attention and response tests).
In contrast, those who napped did markedly better, and actually
improved in their capacity to memorize facts. The difference between
the two at six p.m. was not small: a 20 percent learning advantage for
those who slept.
Having observed that sleep
restores the brains
capacity for learning, making room for new memories, we went in search
of exactly what it was about sleep that transacted
the restoration of benefit. Analyzing the electrical
brainwaves of those in the nap group brought our answer. The memory
refreshment was related to lighter stage 2 NREM sleep, and
specifically the short, powerful bursts called sleep spindles... The
more sleep spindles an individual obtained during the nap the greater
the restoration of their learning when they woke up."
[In
other words a direct one
to one correlation existed between the amount of sleep spindles and the
improvement in learning ability of each subject.]
A different interpretation of the
experiment. This
above is a beautiful
well conducted experiment but this site holds that there may be a
better way of interpreting the experiment's results. This site holds
that it is more likely that memories are not kept in a single place
like the hippocampus but rather each memory is more likely to be a vast
network or web of connections scattered across many brain areas. So,
rather that reducing the significance of this experiment this
site holds that the experiment works even better if in the
experiment memories are not moved from the hippocampus to the
cortex but rather they are part of a path reconstruction
involving the accessing of those memories. Although the experiment set
out to find if memories were moved from the hippocampus to the cortex
the findings of the experiment do not conclusively prove that such a
movement of memory took place. Rather they prove that the memories are
accessed through different pathways after NREM sleep.
In
her book "The Secret World of Sleep" Penelope Lewis an analogy gives
some idea how the above might be possible:
"The analogy [of
memory recall] with
music is actually quite useful, especially if you think of the cortex
as a piano keyboard with different keys representing different sensory
inputs. The keys are all there to be played all the time, but they only
have noticeable impact when they are pressed, and only a few are
pressed
at any given time. If we think of the memory of running on the beach as
a beautiful piece of music, a standard view of memory would suggest the
score for this music is initially stored in the hippocampus which
triggers to all of the piano keys (areas of the neocortex) in the
correct order and with the correct timing to to produce the
music."
Elsewhere
on this site the idea is presented that memories are created by neurons
in the
hippocampus connecting to all the areas active in the cortex at any one
time. Similarly those same neurons could activate the same
areas
of the cortex that were active at
the time of memory storage to recall a memory. They would do this by
means of the many neurons that link
the hippocampus to every part of the cortex. Thus what could be stored
in the hippocampus would not be memories as such but rather small
programs to play memories. The memories are actually anywhere
until they are played when they exist momentarily in the cortex.

REMEMBERING
& NREM SLEEP.
The
function of sleeping after
learning.
Which
provides memory enhancement sleeping or waking? In 1924
two German scientists John Jenkins and Karl Dallenbach showed
conclusively for the first time that that humans remembered far more
after sleeping than they did if they remained awake: Walker refers to
the study as follows:
"Their
study participants first learned a list of verbal facts Thereafter, the
researchers tracked how quickly the participants forgot those memories
over an eight hour interval, either spent awake or across a night of
sleep. Time spent asleep helped cement the newly learned chunks of
information,
preventing them from fading away, an equivalent time spent awake was
deeply hazardous to recently acquired memories resulting in an
accelerated trajectory of forgetting."
These
experimental results have been replicated may times with a memory
retention benefit of between 20 and 40 percent as compared to the same
time spent awake. Sleep
greatly out performs staying awake.
During what stage of sleep is
memory enhanced NREM or REM? In the 1950's the first
experiments were carried out to discover when and in what stages of
sleep this memory improvement was taking place. Walker describes the
study as follows.
"After having learned lists
of facts, researchers allowed participants the opportunity to sleep
only for the first half of the night. In this way, both experimental
groups obtained the same total amount of sleep (albeit short), yet the
former group's was rich in deep NREM, and the latter was dominated
instead by REM... For fact based, textbook-like memory, the result was
clear. It was early night sleep rich in deep NREM that won out in
terms of providing superior memory retention savings relative to to
late-night, REM-rich sleep.
Investigations in the early
2000s, arrived at
a similar conclusion using a different approach. Having learned a list
of facts before bed, participants were allowed to sleep a full eight
hours, recorded with electrodes placed on the head. The next morning
the
participants performed a memory test. When the researchers correlated
the intervening sleep stages with the number of facts retained the
following morning, deep NREM sleep carried the vote: the more deep NREM
sleep, the more information an individual remembered the next day.
Indeed, if you were a participant in such a study, and the only
information I had was the amount of deep NREM sleep you had obtained
that night, I could predict with high accuracy how much you would
remember in the upcoming memory test upon awakening, even
before you took it. That's how deterministic the link between sleep and
memory consolidation can be. [Factual memory
enhancement
occurs in NREM sleep.]

Using MRI scans we have
since looked deep into the brains of of participants to see where those
memories are being retrieved from before sleep and after sleep. It
turns out that those information packets were being recalled from very
different locations within the brain at the two different times. Before
having slept, participants were fetching memories from the short term
storage site of the hippocampus..."
This
statement seems to
contradict some of what this site holds to be true. However, here
Walker
is interpreting areas that are lit up on MRIs and could
merely be showing a lack of activity in the hippocampus after a good
night's sleep and the threads from the hippocampus to the cortex
lighting up before and during sleep. He goes on to say:
"But things were very
different by the next morning. The memories had moved. After a full
night of sleep participants were now receiving that same information
from the neocortex..."
The
neocortex is the area of the brain just below the cortex and it
connects various parts of the cortex together. This site holds that the
connections in the neocortex make up memories in the same way as in the
hippocampus. However the connections from the hippocampus to the
cortex preexist and the ones in the neocortex have to be developed by
the emergence of new synapses. This may sometimes happen overnight as
seems to be the case here or it may sometimes take a long while as with
very complicated memories connecting parts of the brain that are very
far apart. Walker continues:
"Sleep is constantly modifying
the information architecture of the brain at night. Even daytime naps
as
short as twenty minutes can offer a memory consolidation
advantage, so long as they contain enough NREM sleep."
 
Walker has looked at many
many sleep studies young kid participants, adolescent participants,
midlife participants, and 40-60 year old participants and all their
memories improved with deep NREM sleep.
Walker continues like this:
"At
every stage of human life, the relationship between NREM sleep
and
memory solidification is therefore observed. It's not just humans
either. Studies in chimpanzees, bonobos, and orangutans have
demonstrated that all three groups are better able to remember where
food items have been placed in their environments after they sleep.
Descend down the plylogenetic chain to cats, rats, and even insects,
and
the memory-maintaining benefits of NREM sleep remains on powerful
display."
Walker
created many other
experiments with much longer and more varied sequences and obtained a
similar result each time. Walker goes on to
enlighten us about further findings in the same experiments above:
"Perhaps
more remarkable as we analyzed the
sleep-spindle bursts of activity we observed a strikingly
reliable loop of electrical current pulsing throughout the brain that
repeated every 100 to 200 milliseconds. The pulses kept weaving a path
back and forth between the hippocampus with its short-term limited
storage space, and the far larger long-term storage site of the cortex."
What
if the the loop of current traveling between the hippocampus and the
cortex was merely an indication that the memory in question was being
activated by means of links connected to the hippocampus. Further
suppose that after a goodnight of NREM sleep the same memory was
activated by means of connections in the cortex.
It would
not be necessary for the memory to move at all. The only thing that
would need to change would be the
way in which the memory was activated. Suppose during the night a path
of synapses through the cortex, that already connected the memory in
question, was somehow
made more likely than the connection though the hippocampus. This may
seem like a lot of 'what ifs' and 'supposition' and but it would free
up
pathways to and from the hippocampus to the cortex to be freed up to
form new memories that are separate from the existing ones.
Why
would the brain need two ways of reaching a memory? This is likely for
the same reason that computers often have a number of different ways of
doing the same thing. Redundancy in any system is always a plus. But
the experiments by Walker and his team were of huge importance because
they proved conclusively that something happens during NREM sleep that
enables participants to form new memories that can be easily recalled,
at least in the short term. As Walker concludes:
"Participants
awoke with a refreshed
capacity to absorb new information..."
Matthew
Walker then goes on
to discuss another similar experiment his team performed later. They
tested
elderly people counting the number of spindles that occurred while they
slept and then gave them similar learning experiences to those in the
experiment above and then tested their retention to see if there was a
correlation between the spindles and their ability to learn. Again this
turned out to be the case. A lack of spindles before trying to memorize
led to a proportional inability to recall. Matthew Walker sums up a whole range of
experiments like these as follows:
"If
you were a participant
in such a study and the only information that I had was the amount of
deep NREM sleep you had obtained that night, I could predict with high
accuracy how much you would remember in the upcoming memory test upon
awakening, even before you took it. That's how deterministic the link
between sleep and memory consolidation can be."
Matthew
Walker continues then indicating
that before having
slept the greatest amount of MRI recorded activity was taking place
in the the subjects hippocampus while after NREM sleep the MRI recorded
activity took place mostly in cortex areas. Walker concludes that this
means that memories are being moved from the hippocampus (short term
memory) to the cortex (long term memory). However it is just as
possible that the memories did not move at all but rather what changed
was the manner in which they were accessed.
SELECTIVE
FORGETTING &
NREM SLEEP.
The
function of initiating selective forgetting.
In
his book
"Why We Sleep"
Matthew Walker set about creating an experiment to determine if humans
had any kind of conscious control over the process of embedding
some memories in a more accessible and more permanent form while
deleting or making other memories less accessible. He describes this
experiment as follows:
"We
designed an experiment that again used daytime naps. At midday, our
research subjects studied a long list of words presented on the
screen, however, a large green "R" or a large red "F" was displayed,
indicating to the participant that they should remember the prior word
(R) or forget the prior word (F). It is not dissimilar to being in a
class and, after being told a fact, the teacher impresses upon you that
it is especially important to remember that information for the exam,
or instead that they made an error and the fact was incorrect, or the
fact will not be tested on the exam, so you don't need to worry about
remembering it for the test. We were effectively doing the same thing
for each word right after learning, tagging it with the label 'to be
remembered' or 'to be forgotten.'

Half
of the participants were then allowed a ninety-minute afternoon nap,
while the other half remained awake. At 6 p.m. we tested everyone's
memory for all the words. We told participants that regardless of the
tag previously associated with a word - to be remembered or to be
forgotten - they should try to recall as many words as possible. Our
question was this: Does sleep improve the retention of all words
equally or does sleep obey the waking command only to remember some
items while forgetting others, based on the tags we had connected to
each?

The
results were clear. Sleep powerfully, yet very selectively, boosted the
retention of those words previously tagged for 'remembering' yet
actively
avoided the strengthening of those memories tagged for 'forgetting.'
Participants who did not sleep showed no such impressive parsing and
differential saving of the memories. ...sleep does not offer a general,
nonspecific (and hence verbose) preservation of all the information you
learn during the day. Instead, sleep is able to offer a far
more
discerning hand in memory improvement: one that preferentially picks
and chooses what information is to ,and is not , ultimately
strengthened. Sleep accomplishes this by using meaningful tags that
have been hung on those memories during initial learning, or
potentially identified during sleep itself. Numerous studies have shown
a similarly intelligent form of of sleep-dependent memory selections
memory selection across both daytime naps and a full night of sleep.
When
we analyzed the sleep records of those individuals who napped, we
gained another insight. Contrary to Francis Crick's prediction, it was
not REM sleep that was sifting through the list of prior words,
separating out those that should be retained and those that should be
removed. Rather, it was NREM sleep, and especially the very quickest of
the sleep spindles that helped bend apart the curves of of remembering
and forgetting."

Walker
and his team performed many other experiments. A good number of
such experiments produced results that showed that specific memories
could be made more likely to be remembered by inducing replay of those
specific memories during NREM sleep.
GENERAL
RECALL IMPROVEMENT BY
MEANS OF ELECTRICAL BRAIN STIMULATION IN
NREM SLEEP.
Electrical
stimulation.
Just
as a computer hard drive often has some files that have become
corrupted and inaccessible, the human brain also has memories that
become
partially damaged or inaccessible. In a brain this means that either
the
pathways to the memory or the pathways that make up the memory have
withered through disuse or have been physically damaged. This is
nothing
new for the brain as most recall seems to require the brain to
reconstruct memories from the clues left behind. It is also well known
from memory studies that recalling memories just before they
are
to be forgotten or when these connections are weakening will cause the
memory to be revitalized as the connections are reinforced,
consolidated and or
rebuilt.
It is likely then that during NREM dreams that memories are recalled in
a similar manner with synapses being both strengthened and or
reconstructed.

Since
sleep is expressed in patterns of electrical brain wave activity it
occurred to some researchers that it might be possible to increase the
amount of recall the brain was doing by flooding the brain with small
electrically charged pulses that are synchronized with the slow waves
of
deep NREM sleep. Walker describes the study as follows:
"In
2006 a research team in Germany recruited a group of healthy young
adults for a pioneering study in which they applied electrodes pads
onto
the head, front and back. Rather than recording the electrical
brainwaves being emitted from the brain during sleep the scientists did
the opposite: inserted small amounts of electrical voltage. They
patiently waited until each participant had entered into the deepest
stages of NREM sleep and, at that point, switched on the brain
stimulator, pulsing in rhythmic time with the slow waves. The
electrical
pulsations were so small that participants did not feel them, nor did
they wake up. But they had a measurable impact on sleep.
Both the size of the slow
brainwaves and the
number of sleep spindles riding on top of the the deep brainwaves were
increased by the stimulation, relative to a control group of subjects
who did not receive stimulation during sleep. Before being put to bed,
all the participants had learned a list of new facts. They were tested
the next morning after sleep. By boosting the electrical quality of
deep-sleep brainwave activity, the researchers almost doubled the
number of facts that individuals were able to recall the following
day, relative to those participants who received no
stimulation."
GENERAL
RECALL IMPROVEMENT BY
MEANS OF OTHER BRAIN STIMULATION IN NREM SLEEP.
Sound
stimulation.
Walker
goes on to describe a number of other methods of stimulation that have
been stimulate these same deep-sleep brainwaves as follows:
"One technology involves quiet auditory tones
being played over speakers next to the sleeper. Like a metronome in
rhythmic stride with the individual slow waves, the tick-tock tones are
precisely synchronized with the individual's sleeping brainwaves to
help entrain their rhythm and produce deeper sleep.

Relative to a
control group that slept but had no synchronous auditory chimes at
night,
the auditory stimulation increased the power of the slow brainwaves and
returned an impressive 40 percent memory enhancement the next morning.

Walker
goes on to warn people not to try to do this at home as safe
technologies are not available and it is easy to cause
yourself
damage trying to synchronize brainwave with either electrical pulses or
sound pulses.

It
may well be that even without individual slow waves becoming
synchronized
with a sound that simply paying a piece of music on a loop all night
after studying with that piece of music playing may also produce good
recall the next day as seems to be the case with odors in the
experiment below.
Odor simulation.
Other
research suggests that memory reactivation can be biased by external
cues presented during NREM sleep
that had been present during prior encoding. The researchers, used odor
cues to trigger memory reactivation processes during sleep in humans.
Participants were asked to encode locations of picture pairs shown on a
computer screen (similar to the game concentration), while they smelled
the scent of roses. During subsequent slow wave sleep participants were
re-exposed to this odor or an odorless alternative. Those participants
who
had smelled the odor during learning and during slow wave sleep showed
increased recall of the card locations at retrieval compared to the
alternative condition. Memory was not improved if participants received
the
odor only during sleep but not during prior encoding, and odor
re-exposure was also not effective during wakefulness
and post-learning REM sleep.
Movement
or rocking simulation.
Following experiments on mice yet another type of stimulation was tried
to influence these deep-sleep brainwaves and so increase memory
retention or recall as Walker summarizes here:
"[A]...Swiss research team recently
suspended a bedframe on ropes from the sealing of a sleep laboratory...
Affixed to one side of the suspended bed was a rotating
pulley. It allowed the researchers to sway the bed from side to side a
controlled speeds. Volunteers then took a nap in the bed as
researchers recorded their sleeping brainwaves. In half the
participants, the researchers gently rocked the bed once they entered
NREM sleep. In the other half of the subjects the bed remained static,
offering a control condition. Slow rocking increased the depth of deep
sleep, boosted the quality of slow brainwaves, and more than doubled
the number of sleep spindles. It is not known whether these sway
induced sleep changes enhance memory, since the researchers did not
perform any such tests with their participants. Nevertheless the
findings offer a scientific explanation for the ancient practice of
rocking a child back and forth in one's arms, or in a crib, inducing
deep sleep."
Of
mice and men. The
other experiment was described at another site as follows:
"The
second study in mice by other researchers
is the first to explore whether rocking promotes sleep in other
species. And,
indeed, it did. The researchers used commercial reciprocating shakers
to rock
the cages of mice as they slept.
While
the best rocking frequency for mice was
found to
be four times faster than in people, the researchers' studies show that
rocking
reduced the time it took to fall asleep and increased sleep time in
mice as it
does in humans. However, the mice did not show evidence of sleeping
more
deeply."
SPECIFIC
RECALL IMPROVEMENT BY
MEANS OF SENSORY BRAIN STIMULATION IN
NREM SLEEP.
Clearly
Walker had shown that memories could be improved by various types of
external simulation during sleep but these all improved memories as a
whole address the idea that people might like to control
which memories are forgotten and which are remembered.
Of sounds and specific items to
recall. and Just
suppose you could decide what items you wished to be able to recall and
be able to place your order before you go to sleep. Next morning you
would awake to find you can recall the items you specified and not any
other items on a list that where you learned the ones specified. An
experiment has been performed that indicates it may be possible to move
memory research in that direction. Walker explains as follows:
"Before
going to sleep, we show participants pictures of objects at different
spacial locations on a computer screen, such as a cat in the lower
right side, or a bell in the upper center, or a kettle near the top
right of the screen. As a participant you have to remember not only the
individual items you have been shown, but also their spacial location
on the screen. You will be shown a hundred of these items. After sleep,
picture objects will again appear on the screen, now in the center,
some of which you have seen before, some you have not. You have to
decide if you remember the picture object or not, and if you do you
must move that picture object to the spacial location on the screen
where it originally appeared using a mouse.

But
here is the intriguing twist. As you were originally learning the
images before sleep, each time an object was presented on the screen a
corresponding sound was played. For example, you would hear 'meow' when
the cat picture was shown, or ding-a-ling when the bell was shown. All
picture objects are paired, or 'auditory-tagged,' with a
semantically matching sound. When you are asleep and in NREM sleep
specifically, an experimenter will will replay half of the previously
tagged sounds (fifty of the total hundred) to you sleeping
brain
at low volume using speakers either side of the bed. As if helping
guide the brain in a targeted search-and-retrieve effort, we can
trigger the selective reactivation of corresponding individual
memories,
prioritizing them for sleep strengthening, relative to those that were
not reactivated during NREM sleep.

When
you are tested the following morning, you will have a quite remarkable
bias in your recollection, remembering far more of the items that we
reactivated during sleep using the sound cues than those not
reactivated. Note that all one hundred of the original memory items
passed through sleep. However using sound cuing, we avoid
indiscriminate
enhancement of all that you have learned. Analogous to looping your
favorite song in a repeating playlist at night, we cherry pick specific
slices of your autobiographical past and preferentially strengthen them
by using the individualized sound cues during sleep."
It
seems clear that what is happening is the sound cues are causing the
sleeping NREM deep-sleep brains to recall specific memories. As we well
know memories recalled automatically direct our brains to prioritize
memories for placement in long term memory. The likelihood of recall is
then greatly increased.
It
seems possible that almost any sound could be associated with the
pictures in the above experiment and the experiment would still produce
positive results. This is using the same principle as as Pavlov in his
classical conditioning only it triggers the memory during NREM sleep
instead of while the subject is awake. Obviously though, the use of
sounds that are already linked to or associated with the pictures
guaranteed that the experiment would work well.
One
can imagine whole study sessions at schools and colleges having
specific music played while they are studying a specific topic and the
giving each student a copy of the music to take home that night so they
can replay the music quietly while they are in NREM sleep. This could
produce startling exam results at exam time especially if the music was
played during NREM sleep the night before the exam.
MOTOR
SKILL LEARNING ENHANCEMENT
DURING NREM SLEEP.
Overnight improvement. On
awakening, I could just do it. Walker
tells the
story of being
approached by a pianist who's remarks set him on a path to creating
experiments to study this possibility. Walker relates the those remarks
as follows:
"He
said he was intrigued by my
description of sleep as an active brain state, one in which we may
renew
and even strengthen those things we have previously learned. Then came
a comment that would leave me reeling and trigger a major focus of my
research for years to come. 'As a pianist' he said 'I have an
experience that seems too frequent to be chance. I will be practicing a
particular piece, even late into the evening, and I cannot seem to
master it. Often, I make the same mistake at the same place in a
particular movement. I go to bed frustrated. But when I wake up the
next morning and sit back down at the piano, I can just play
perfectly.'"
Walker
was faced with the idea that perhaps practice alone was not sufficient
to create improvement in a skill. Perhaps improvement was
only
possible with a combination of practice plus sleep. Gradually
Walker envisioned and created a number of experiments that would
eventually seem to confirm all the pianist's intuitions. The first
experiment of this sort is explained by Walker as follows:
 
"I
took a large group of right handed individuals and had them learn to
type a number sequence on a keyboard with their left hand, such as
4-1-3-2-4 as quickly and as accurately as possible. Like learning a
piano scale, subjects practiced the motor skill sequence over and over
again, for a total of twelve minutes, taking short breaks throughout.
Unsurprisingly, the participants improved in their performance across
the the training session; practice after all, is supposed to make
perfect. We then tested the participants twelve hours later. Half of
the participants had learned the sequence in the morning and were
tested later that evening after remaining awake across the day. The
other half of the subjects learned the sequence in the evening and we
retested them the next morning after a similar twelve hour delay, but
one that contained a full eight-hour night of sleep.
Those
who remained awake across
the day showed no evidence of a significant improvement in performance.
However, fitting with the pianist's original description, those who
were tested after the same delay of twelve hours, but spanned a night
of sleep, showed a striking 20 percent jump in performance speed and a
near 35 percent improvement in accuracy. Importantly, those
participants who learned the motor skill in the morning - and who
showed no improvement that evening - did go on to show an identical
bump in performance when retested after a further twelve hours, now
after they, too, had had a full night's sleep.
In
other words, your brain will
continue to improve skill memories in the absence of further practice."
It
seems that while some improvement was possible with practice alone a
far greater improvement was obtainable with the addition of a good
night's sleep. Not only that but fluidity and accuracy were also
improved with sleep. Walker goes on to say:
"Their typing post sleep,
was now fluid and unbroken. Gone was the staccato performance, replaced
by seamless automaticity which is the ultimate goal of learning..."
Skills
are both created and improved by sleep. In
other words the pianist's
intuitions were completely born out. What seems to be happening is that
while we are sleeping in a NREM state our brains are carefully weaving
together disparate uncoordinated action elements into a fully
coordinated smooth flowing skill. Or an already acquired skill is being
woven together with newly learned action elements to create a
far improved skill that is even better coordinated.

Skill
learning and & NREM
sleep. In
his book
"Why We Sleep"
Matthew Walker goes on to show that not only can fact learning be
improved by the intervention of NREM sleep but also that habits,
skills
and the smooth flow of automatic
actions
are also created and improved by NREM sleep.
Ultimately
Matthew Walker
tested many of the subjects
of the experiments dealing with motor skill enhancement (explained
above) to find out what was happening in
the subject's brains
while they slept. He explains what he found:
"My
final discovery, in what spanned almost a decade of research,
identified the type of sleep responsible for the overnight
motor-skill enhancement, carrying with it societal and medical lessons.
The increases in speed and accuracy, underpinned by efficient
automaticity, were directly related to the amount of stage 2
NREM, especially in the last two hours of an eight-hour night of sleep
(e.g., from five to seven a.m., should you have fallen asleep at eleven
p.m.). Indeed it was the number of those wonderful sleep spindles in
the last two hours of the late morning - the time of night with the
richest spindle burst of brain wave activity - that were linked with
the off line memory boost."
"More
striking was the fact that the increase of those spindles after
learning was detected only in regions of the scalp that sit above the
motor cortex (just in front of the crown of your head), and not in any
other areas. The greater the local increase in sleep spindles over the
part of the brain we had forced to learn the motor skill exclusively,
the better the performance on awakening... Perhaps more relevant to the
modern world is the time-of-night effect we discovered. Those last two
hours of sleep are precisely the window that many of us feel it is okay
to cut short to get a jump start on the day."
REM
SLEEP
Rapid
Eye Movement dreams.
The father of dream research was want
to say: "Dreaming permits each and every one of us to be
quietly and safely insane every night of our lives." He said
this of course because the main dreams that we remember, although they
sometimes seem to be
logical while we are experiencing them, are actually very illogical,
with transitions and connections that appear to be completely random.
This site holds to the idea that perhaps we need to be crazy every
night. If repetition is so important, why do we spend so much time
dreaming illogical dreams while we are asleep? As expressed elsewhere
this site is
unconvinced that repetition alone is the way in which memories are
consolidated.
Paralysis.
Another feature of REM dreams is the fact that during such dreams we
become paralyzed. While in other stages of sleep we can toss and turn
and move about. But during REM sleep we are paralyzed, and for good
reason. We are in a highly emotional state (intense emotions,
especially of fear, rise and fall chaotically), we are hallucinating
(in
that we are seeing hearing things that are not in the real world) and
we are just plain crazy. On top of that, during this sleep stage all
the
schemas or action programs become available to run. If we were not
paralyzed we would be running, jumping, dancing, fighting and doing god
knows what. In his book
"The Chemistry of Conscious States" J. Allen Hobson explain it
like this:
"The
motor programs in the brain
are never more active than during REM sleep! As our dreams make clear,
REM sleep entails a frenzy of action - we run, we drive, we fly, we
swim. There is no rest in REM sleep for the central programs that move
us about by day. On the contrary, they are souped up and we assume for
good reason: to prevent their decay from disuse, to rehearse for their
future actions when called on during waking, and to embed themselves in
a rich matrix of meaning.

Sometimes
an aberration occurs where people wake from REM sleep, but remain
paralyzed for a while, unable to move. This experience may be
accompanied by residual dream elements, delusions involving people or
things to be feared. This also may be accompanied by returning feelings
of panic and dread.
It
seems likely that one function of this type of dreaming
might be the the strengthening of these action programs by running many
of them without any movement taking place, spinning our wheels (so to
speak) while we sleep. This is what Revonsuo's
threat simulation theory is all about. Think
about it. Many of our dreams concern the fight or flight response.
Keeping programs concerned with fight or flight in good working order
would have important evolutionary consequences.

Early
stage REM dreams.
During the early stages of REM sleep we also appear to have dreams that
more closely resemble the replaying of
experiences or memories from the previous day and other fairly recent
memories. These mixed bits of replays are woven into dreams that tend
to be longer in
duration than slow wave recall, and like all dreams make
little rational sense.
This site holds that these dreams occurring early REM sleep may be a
another type of memory consolidation or
reinforcement. This site has always held
that the main way in which memories are
consolidated is through elaboration. In other words it is not the
repetition itself that enables consolidation of memories, but rather
the new connections that are included during the iteration, that makes
the difference.
While
we are awake
we imprint sensory information as memories but we
are not immediately able to comprehend or understand such information
completely. Initially the
information connects with only a limited amount of the other data
stored already in our minds. The
dreams that occur in this early stage of REM sleep may be our brains
consolidating memories in a manner that is far superior to mere replay.
We may be making connections to the memory from all the other similar
or logically connectible information in our brains. These
numerous
extra connections would make memories both more understandable and more easily accessed. In this way we may be
building a mental map of what we need to understand or learn and indeed
solve problems.

Stitching information into our
map of knowledge in our sleep. While
we sleep and during the early stages of REM sleep our brains may well
be integrating
new information with the old information building a structure that fits
it all together. What research has shown clearly is that after sleep
information is not only better remembered
but better understood or comprehended and more able to be put to use
solving our trickiest problems. This is consistent with our brains
constructing mental maps or maps of reality. In her book
"The
Secret
World of Sleep" sleep researcher Penelope
Lewis puts it like this:
"The
answer appears to be that sleep does a lot more than strengthen
individual memories. It is also involved in the complex process of
integrating new information with old and abstracting out general
principals or rules which describe a corpus of events and help us to
make informed predictions about the future."
"Taken
together these studies demonstrate that sleep is important for
combining information from multiple sources. It helps us to extract
statistical regularities, pull out general principles, integrate new
information with older knowledge structures and piece together a larger
picture from a set of interrelated fragments."
In
his book
"The
Chemistry of Conscious States" J. Allen Hobson suggests
that there may be three different ways in which both early and
latter stages of REM
sleep may use dreaming to integrate new information with older
information in our minds, and in doing so turn it into knowledge:
[Distribution,
Hyperassociation and Proceeduralization.] I call these
additional memory processes distribution, hyperassociation
and proceduralization."
[Knowledge
permanence and
stability.]
"The
representation of the
memory in neuronal networks could be made more secure by its
simultaneous distribution to other networks."
[Knowledge
versatility and
understanding.]
"It could be made more
versatile by linking it in hyperassociative fashion to every network
with which it shares formal features (such as axles with long things,
hard things, strong things)."
[Knowledge
usefulness and
interconnectedness.] "It could be made more useful if
it were
linked to procedures that it served (such as weight bearing, prying).
"Dreaming
has several features
that could enhance all...of these functions... For the memory
redistribution function, REM sleep provides a massive, widespread
activation with intense reiterative stimulation of all the cortical
circuits of the brain. For the hyperassociative function, REM
sleep provides the coactivation of newly sensitized circuits and all
those circuits previously endowed with the multiple interconnections
necessary for category overinclusiveness. For the
proceduralization function, REM sleep provides automatic running of
motor programs that give the data access to existing action files."
Constructing a map of reality in
our sleep. J.
Allen Hobson goes on to speculate as to how this might all fit together
as follows:
"When
we find ourselves in a new place, we at once begin to develop
orientational schemas. We build up our brain-mind maps by incorporating
the results of exploring the environment and moving through it, while
at the same time sniffing it, hearing it, and feeling its textures.
This leads us to an important question that is tougher than it looks:
How can we most efficiently and effectively assure the orientational
experience we perceive gets built into our system? In addition, how can
we be sure that if a built in procedure isn't used, it doesn't get
lost?
One
way would be to have a state (waking) in which we are exposed to new
information and another state (REM sleep) in which the new data is
integrated with the whole set of existing programs in the system. For
that to happen, we would need to run the system automatically for a
considerable length of time each day (say one and a half hours, the
usual grand total of REM sleep for a night); we would want to run it
fast (say six procedures a second, the usual rate of PGO wave signals);
we would want to run it with the clutch disengaged (so the system does
not have to output); and we would want to run it in an altered chemical
climate (to favor a set of molecular operations that differs from that
in waking, to encode long-term memory). That's a tall order. But it's
all done reliably, efficiently and unconsciously in REM sleep, mother
of all procedures. We are performing mental gymnastics, using motor
programs to train our brain each time we dream...
These
startling dream
enactments...are so full of meanings... that REM sleep must be
preparing us for almost any possible waking eventuality. The programs
are all there. Life's events call them forth. And most of the time,
they otherwise stay put."
Latter
stage REM dreams.
However, we may also require
completely illogical connections to memories, or in other words random
connections. Penelope
Lewis says:
"Dreams
occur at all stages of sleep, but they seem to become increasingly
fragmented as the night progresses. ...REM dreams that occur late in
the night are typically much more bizarre and disjointed."
[This sort of illogical elaboration, like logical elaboration, would
provide more pathways though which each memory could be accessed.]
If
a need for random connections could be established it would explain
illogical dreams, it would explain their illogical nature and it would
be
consistent with the above research on the replaying of previous
experiences for the purpose of consolidating learning. It is
possible we have both kinds of nightly experiences. It would be
possible
to have nightly replays of waking experiences with perhaps random
connections thrown in, or the waking experiences could be chopped up
and
reconnected randomly to produce chaotic dream like experiences. In this
way recent waking experiences could be activated in concert
with other random waking experiences to form new connections between
clumps of knowledge at random.
The
random chaos of dreams and creation. The
question then would be why would the brain need random connections? The
answer might be quite simple. One possibility is, that for every part
of the brain to be connected, it may require that some of the
connections are illogical. Another more likely possibility is that all
creative
activity, including problem solving, hunches and incite, need random or
chaotic
connections to function. Without dreams it may be impossible to be
original and creative. In his book
"Where Good Ideas Come From" Steven Johnson puts it like this:
"There
is nothing mystical about the role of dreams in scientific discovery.
While dream activity remains a fertile domain for research, we know
that during REM sleep acetylcholine-releasing cells in the brain stem
fire indiscriminately, sending surges of electricity billowing across
the brain. Memories and associations are triggered in a chaotic,
semirandom fashion, creating the hallucinatory quality of dreams. Most
of those new neuronal connections are meaningless, but every now and
then the dreaming brain stumbles across a valuable link that has
escaped waking consciousness. In this sense, Freud had it backward with
his notion of dreamwork: the dream is not somehow unveiling a repressed
truth. Instead, it is exploring, trying to find new truths by
experimenting with novel combinations of neurons."
In
her book
"The Secret
World of Sleep"
sleep researcher Penelope Lewis shows that random brain stem firings
cannot fully explain dreams because most dreams, especially those
occurring earlier in the night, tend to have logical threads running
through them, and are sometimes persistent in reoccurring.
Nevertheless,
it may well be, that random brain stem firing is responsible for the
chaotic nature of REM dreams occurring later in the night. In any case
Lewis agrees that the chaotic structure of latter dreaming may well
determine creativity. She explains it this way:
"Although
we don't quite understand how dreams achieve this type of innovative
recombination of material, it seems clear that the sleeping brain is
somehow freed of constraints and can thus create whole sequences of
free associations. This is not only useful for creativity, it is also
thought to facilitate insight and problem solving. It may even be
critical for the integration of newly acquired memories with more
remote ones... In fact, this facilitated lateral thinking could, in
itself, be the true purpose of dreams. It is certainly valuable enough
to have evolved through natural selection."
These
random connections could be understood to produce occasional errors in
thought that in turn produce crazy ideas from which we can mine useful
novel
ideas. Without these chaotic dreams thoughts may get stuck in ruts with
no escape
route.
Experimental
findings about REM sleep.
In
his book
"Why We Sleep"
Matthew Walker also ran a number of experiments that relate learning to
REM sleep. For a start he explains that during REM sleep the brain is
very active. He points out that MRI studies show four sections of the
brain are particularly active during REM sleep as follows:
"(1)
the visuospacial regions at the back of the
brain, which enable complex visual perception...
(2)
the motor cortex, which instigates movement...
(3)
the hippocampus and surrounding regions...which
support autobiographical memory...
(4)
the deep emotional centers of the brain - the amygdala and the
cingulate cortex, a ribbon of tissue that sits above the amygdala and
lines the inner surface of your brain - both of which help to generate
and process emotions..."

REM
DREAMS DECODED.
This
information is consistent with dreams that are visual, active,
biographical, and highly emotional but not inconsistent with dreams
that are illogical and randomly chaotic. As should have also been
expected certain other brain areas were found during REM sleep to be
disconnected and suppressed. Walker explains:
"What
came as a surprise, however, was the pronounced deactivation of other
brain regions - specifically, circumscribed regions of the far left and
far right sides of the prefrontal cortex. ...these neural territories
had become markedly suppressed in activity during the otherwise highly
active state of REM sleep... This region...manages rational thought and
logical decision making... [and] otherwise maintains your
cognitive capacity for ordered logical thought..."
From
these findings Walker and his staff were able to predict some of the
general content of dreams which they were then able to check by waking
and asking the subjects what they were dreaming about. By the extent of
activity in each of the four areas they were able to predict how
emotional the dreams were, how much activity was involved, how
connected or chaotic it was, and much visual information was being
processed.
THE
CONTENT OF DREAMS.
It
is important to note that the content of dreams has long been
thought difficult or impossible to predict from brain scans
that
were taken during the dreaming. However, it may well be possible to
predict rudimentary elements in dreams from brain scans. This allows
researchers to have some idea of what the dream content of a dreamer
may be without having to resort to waking the dreamer and asking them.
"In
2013, a research team in Japan led by Dr. Yukiyasu Kamitani at the
Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International in Kyoto,
found an ingenious way to address the question. They essentially
cracked the code of an individual's dream for the very first time and,
in doing so, led us to an ethically uncomfortable place.
Individuals
in the experiment consented to the study - an important fact as we
shall see. The results remain preliminary, since they were obtained in
just three individuals. But they were highly significant. Also the
researchers focused on the short dreams we frequently have just at the
moment when we are falling asleep, rather than the dreams of REM sleep,
though the method will soon be applied to REM sleep.
The
scientists placed each participant into an MRI scanner numerous times
over the course of several days. Every time the participant fell
asleep,
the researchers would wait for a short while as they recorded the brain
activity, and then wake the person up and obtain a dream report. They
would let the person fall back to sleep, and repeat the procedure. The
researchers continued to do this until they had gathered hundreds of
dream reports and corresponding snapshots of brain activity from their
participants. An example of of one of the dream reports was: 'I saw a
big bronze statue...on a small hill, and below the hill there were
houses, streets, and trees.'
Kamitani
and his team then distilled
all the dream reports down into twenty core
content categories that were most frequent in the dreams
of these
individuals, such as books, cars, furniture, computers, men, women,and
food.

The scientists were able to
predict with significant accuracy the content of participant's dreams
at any one moment in time using just the MRI scans, operating
completely blind to the dream reports of the participants. Using the
template data from the MRI images , they could tell if you were
dreaming of a man or a woman, a dog or a bed, flowers or a knife.
 REM SLEEP AS REPLAY OF
EMOTIONAL
THEMES.   
There
has been some expectation that researchers should find daytime
experiences being reexperienced during REM sleep as a kind of
reinforcement of those experiences. This turns out not to be the case.
While it seems that very short elements of such experience occur during
NREM sleep very little repetition of waking experience occurs during
REM sleep. In
his book
"Why We Sleep"
Matthew Walker goes on to document the findings of Robert Stickgold who
undertook to discover if a significant amount of REM dream content
concerned replay of previous waking autobiographical experiences. He
describes Stickgold's experiment as follows:
"For
two weeks straight, he had twenty-nine healthy young adults keep a
detailed log of daytime activities, the events they engaged in
(going to work, meeting specific friends, meals they ate, sports they
played, etc.) and their current emotional concerns. In addition he had
them keep dream journals, asking them to write down any recalled dreams
that they had when they awoke up each morning. He then had external
judges systematically compare the reports of the participants' waking
activities with their dream reports, focusing on the degree of
similarity of well-defined features such as locations, actions,
objects, characters, themes, and emotions.
Of
a total of 299 dream reports that Stickgold collected from these
individuals across fourteen days, a clear rerun of prior waking life
events - day residue - was found in just 1 or 2 percent... But
Stickgold did find a strong and predictive daytime signal in the static
of nighttime dream reports: emotions. Between 35 and 55 percent of
emotional themes and concerns that participants were having while they
were awake during the day powerfully and unambiguously resurfaced in
the dreams they were having at night."
Basically
it was
clear that NREM dreams were where actions and facts were selected for
storage or deletion (by being recalled or not), while REM dreams were
where emotions were explored.
While it is possible that these highly emotional dreams perform
functions like preparing us for unknown situations and solving problems
this was not what Walker and his team decided to investigate initially.
The
functions of REM sleep and
dreams.
REM
SLEEP AS OVERNIGHT THERAPY. 
While
thinking about the old adage "Time heals all wounds." Walker wondered
if this might only be true if sleep was involved. He wondered if REM
sleep might be performing overnight therapy to allow the dreamer to be
able to deal with harsh emotions. Maybe REM dreams replayed emotions so
they remained in memory as knowledge but without the experiencing of
toxic, traumatic
emotions. Firstly he may have discovered or maybe it was all ready well
known that (as he explains):
"Concentrations
of a key stress-related chemical called noradrenaline are completely
shut off within your brain when you enter this dreaming sleep state. In
fact REM sleep is the only time during the twenty-four hour period when
your brain is completely devoid of this anxiety-triggering molecule.
Noradrenaline, also known as as norepinephrine, is the brain equivalent
of to a body chemical you already know and have felt the effects of:
adrenaline (epinephrine)."
Walker
then goes on to
speculate what this lack of
noradrenaline might mean:
"I
therefore wondered whether the brain during REM sleep was reprocessing
upsetting memory experiences and themes in this neurochemically calm
(low noradrenaline), 'safe' dreaming brain environment. Is the
REM-sleep dreaming state a perfectly designed soothing balm - one that
removes the emotional sharp edges of our emotional lives? It seemed so
from everything neurobiology and neurophysiology was telling us (me).
If so we should awake feeling better about distressing events of the
day(s) prior.

This
was the theory of overnight therapy. It postulated that the process of
REM-sleep dreaming accomplishes two critical goals: (1) sleeping to
remember the details of those valuable, salient experiences,
integrating them with existing knowledge and putting them into
autobiographical perspective, yet (2) sleeping to forget, or dissolve,
the visceral, painful emotional charge that had previously
been
wrapped around those memories."

The
idea was that just as the emotional intensity of memories does in fact
subside as time goes bye. (You can (as you know) accurately relive
memories without experiencing the intense visceral emotions, the toxic
pain and mental anguish originally embedded in them.) There
had
to
be a reason why it happens and a process that explained how it happens.
Walker goes on:
"The
theory argued that we have REM-sleep dreaming to thank for
this
palliative dissolving of emotion from experience.
Through its therapeutic work at night, REM sleep performed the elegant
trick of divorcing the bitter emotional rind from the information rich
fruit. We can therefore learn and usefully recall salient life events
without being crippled by the emotional baggage that those painful
experiences originally carried."
So
Walker and his team
designed
an experiment to test this theory and discover its
viability:
"We
recruited a collection of healthy young adults and randomly assigned
them to two groups. Each group viewed a set of emotional
images
while in an MRI scanner as we measured their emotional brain
reactivity. During these two exposure sessions separated by twelve
hours, participants also rated how emotional they felt in response to
each image.
Importantly,
however, half the participants viewed the images in the morning and
again in the evening, being awake between the two viewings. The other
half of the participants viewed the images in the evening and again the
next morning after a full night of sleep. In this way we could measure
what their brains were objectively telling us using the MRI scans, and
in addition, what the participants themselves were subjectively feeling
about the relived experiences, having had a night of seep in between or
not.

Those
who slept in between the two sessions reported a significant decrease
in how emotional they were feeling in response to seeing those images
again. In addition, results of the MRI scans showed a large and
significant reduction in reactivity in the amygdala, that emotional
center of the brain that creates painful feelings. Moreover, there was
a reengagement of the rational prefrontal cortex of the brain after
sleep that was helping maintain a dampening brake influence on
emotional reactions. In contrast, those who remained awake across the
day without the chance to sleep and digest those experiences showed no
such dissolving of emotional reactivity over time. Their deep emotional
brain reactions were just as strong and negative, if not more so, at
the second viewing compared to the first, and they reported a similar
powerful reexperiencing of painful feelings to boot.
Since
we had recorded the sleep of of each participant during the intervening
night between the two test sessions, we could answer a follow-up
question: Is there something about the type or quality of sleep that an
individual experiences that predict how successful sleep is at
accomplishing the next-day emotional resolution?
As
the theory predicted, it was the dreaming state of REM-sleep - and
specific patterns of electrical activity that reflected the drop in
stress related brain chemistry during the dream state - that determined
the success of overnight therapy from one individual to the next. It
was not, therefor, time per se that healed all wounds, but instead it
was time spent in dream sleep that was providing emotional
convalescence. To sleep, perchance to heal."
Walker
had proved his
theory, in that clearly REM
sleep provided some benefit in lowering toxic emotional experiences in
relived memories while retaining the useful knowledge in those
memories. But how could depression and PTSD be explained or how did
they fit with this theory?
Depression
and the theory of overnight therapy. The work of a Dr
Cartwright provided some
answers about depression as Walker explains:
"Cartwright
demonstrated that it was only those patients who were expressly
dreaming
about the painful experiences around the time of the events
[that
caused the depression] who
went on to gain clinical resolution from
their despair, mentally recovering a year later as clinically
determined by having no identifiable depression. Those who were
dreaming, but not dreaming of the painful experience itself, could not
get past the event, still being dragged down by a strong undercurrent
of depression that remained.

Cartwright
had shown that it was not enough to have REM sleep, or even
generic dreaming, when it comes to resolving our emotional past. Her
patients required REM sleep with dreaming, but dreaming of a very
specific kind: that which expressly involved dreaming about emotional
themes and sentiments of the walking trauma. It was only that content
specific form of dreaming that was able to accomplish clinical
remission and offer emotional closure in these patients, allowing them
to move forward into a new emotional future, and not be enslaved by a
traumatic past."
PTSD
and the theory of overnight therapy. Walker
started conjecturing possibilities about
PTSD:
"I
wondered whether the REM-sleep overnight therapy mechanism we had
discovered in healthy individuals had broken down in people suffering
from PTSD, thereby failing to help them deal with their trauma memories
effectively... Most compelling to me, however, were the repetitive
nightmares reported in PTSD patients - a symptom so reliable that it
forms part of the list of features required for diagnosis of the
condition.
If
the brain cannot divorce the emotion from memory across the first night
following the trauma experience, the theory suggests that a repeat
attempt of emotional memory stripping will occur on the second night,
as the strength of the 'emotional tag' associated with the memory
remains too high. If the process fails a second time, the same attempt
will continue to repeat the next night, and the next night, like a
broken record. This was precisely what appeared to be happening with
the recurring nightmares of the trauma experience in PTSD patients.

A
testable prediction emerged: If I could lower the levels of
noradrenaline in the brains of PTSD patients during sleep, thereby
reinstating the right chemical conditions for sleep to do its trauma
therapy work, then I should be able to restore healthier quality REM
sleep. With that restored REM-sleep should come an improvement in the
clinical symptoms of PTSD, and further, a decrease in the frequency of
painful repetitive nightmares. It was a scientific theory in search of
clinical evidence."
A
new drug to improve PTSD.
Shortly after Walker had published this theory he had a chance meeting
with Dr. Murry Raskind at a conference and uncovered his clinical
evidence. In his book Walker goes on:
"At
the conference Raskind presented recent findings that were perplexing
to him. In his PTSD clinic, Raskind had been treating his war veteran
patients with a drug called prazosin to manage their high blood
pressure. While the drug was somewhat effective in lowering blood
pressure in the body Raskind found it had a far more powerful yet
entirely unexpected benefit within the brain: it alleviated
the reoccurring nightmares in his PTSD patients. After only a
few
weeks of
treatment, his patients would return to the clinic and, with puzzled
amazement, say things like, 'Doc its the strangest thing, my dreams
don't have
those flashbacks nightmares any more. I feel better, less scared to
fall asleep at night.'"

It
turns out that the drug Prazosin, which Raskind was prescribing simply
to lower blood pressure, also had the fortuitous side effect of
suppressing noradrenaline in the brain. Raskind had delightfully and
inadvertently conducted the experiment I was trying to conceive of
myself. He had created precisely the neurochemical condition - a
lowering of the abnormally high concentrations of stress-related
noradrenaline - within the brain during REM sleep that had been absent
for so long in in these PTSD patients."

REM
SLEEP TO DECODE WAKING EXPERIENCES.
Reading body language and facial
expressions.
During
our waking daytime experience we take in sensory data or information.
It seems that during the night while we sleep this information is
somehow connected to what we already know and in being so connected
becomes what we call knowledge. This knowledge is critical in being
able
to predict what to do in various scenarios. The most important part of
this knowledge is being able to predict other humans or as it is
sometimes called "theory of mind".
Walker
and his colleagues began to wonder if REM sleep might play a part in
the
function of this theory of mind. One of the first types of learning
they tested
was
our ability to read facial expressions and determine human intentions.
Our ability to predict outcomes and the probabilities of
likely
future events dealing with many types of dangers is contingent on our
ability to correctly interpret various emotional expressions in other
humans and animals. Here is what Walker and his team did:
"Participants
came to my laboratory and had a full night of sleep.The following
morning we showed them many pictures of a specific individuals face.
However no two pictures were the same. Instead the facial expression of
that one individual varied across the images in a gradient, shifting
from friendly (with a slight smile, calming eye aperture, and
approachable look) to increasingly stern and threatening (pursed lips,
a
furrowed brow, and a menacing look in the eyes). Each image of this
individual was subtly different from those on either side of it on the
emotional gradient, and across tens of pictures, the full range of
intent was expressed, from very prosocial (friendly) to strongly
antisocial (unfriendly).

Participants
viewed the faces in a random fashion while we scanned their brains
in an MRI machine, and they rated how approachable or threatening the
images were. The MRI scans allowed us to measure how their brains were
interpreting and accurately parsing the threatening facial expressions
from the friendly ones after having had a full night of sleep. All the
participants repeated the same experiment, but this time we deprived
them of sleep, including the critical stage of REM. Half the
participants went through the sleep deprivation session first, followed
by the sleep session second, and vice versa. In each session, a
different individual was featured in the pictures, so there was no
memory or repetition effects.

Having
had a full night of sleep, which contained REM sleep, participants
demonstrated a beautifully precise tuning curve of emotional face
recognition, rather like a stretched out V shape. When navigating the
cornucopia of facial expressions we showed them inside the MRI scanner,
their brains had no problem deftly separating one emotion from another
across the delicately changing gradient, and the accuracy of their own
ratings proved this to be similarly true. It was effortless to
disambiguate friendly and approachable signals from those intimating
even minor threat as the emotional tide changed toward the foreboding.
Confirming
the importance of the dream state, the better the quality of
REM
sleep form one individual to the next across that rested night, the
more precise the tuning within the emotional decoding networks of the
brain the next day. Through this platinum-grade nocturnal service,
better REM-sleep quality at night provided superior comprehension of
the social world the next day."
Unlike
many of the other experiments Walker presents in his book, this one
does not pin down how REM sleep might might function to help decode
waking experiences and the experiment does not prove that it does. It
does, however prove that a lack of REM sleep will definitely cause this
function to deteriorate. Walker continues:
"But
when those same participants were deprived of sleep, including the
essential influence of REM sleep, they could no longer distinguish one
emotion from another with accuracy. The tuning V of the brain had been
rudely pulled all the way up from the base and flattened into a
horizontal line, as if the brain was in a state of generalized
hypersensitivity without the ability to map gradations of
emotional
signals from the outside world. Gone was the precise ability to read
giveaway clues in another's face. The brain's emotional navigation
system had lost its true magnetic north of directionality and
sensitivity: a compass that otherwise guides us toward numerous
evolutionary advantages.
With
the absence of such emotional acuity, normally gifted by by the
re-tuning skills of REM sleep at night, the sleep-deprived participants
slipped into a default of fear bias, believing even gentle - or
somewhat friendly looking faces were menacing. The outside world had
become a more threatening and adverse place when the brain lacked REM -
untruthfully so. Reality and perceived reality were no longer the same
in the 'eyes' of the sleepless brain. By removing REM sleep we had
quite literally, removed participants' levelheaded ability to read the
social world around them."
Walker
does not try to speculate why REM sleep would be able to help us
interpret emotional signaling. However, Hobsen's ideas on the
possibility of REM dreams integrating new information to become more
permanent and stable, more variable and understandable, more useful and
interconnected, could offer such an explanation. Unfortunately Walkers
experiments do not try to implicate such functions or test these ideas.
This site is well aware that many
brain and bodily functions deteriorate if we do not get enough sleep.
The brain may simply function badly in general if we do not get enough
REM sleep.

CREATIVE
INCUBATION VIA REM SLEEP AND DREAMS. 
In
his book "Why We Sleep" Matthew Walker set out to show that REM
dreaming enables creativity in a number of different ways. He suggests
that these dreams enable creativity in several similar but distinct
ways that serve very different creative functions. One function creates
new and unique ideas by squishing bits of unrelated memory together
while another solves problems by mixing random memory elements with the
problem to see if any of it produces some possible hint of a solution.
Or dreams may place the problem in various unconnected but similar or
metaphorically compatible contexts.
Creative
ideas dreamed
in REM sleep.
While
it would be impossible to create a study to show that sleeping and
dreaming produce creative ideas and invent new creative works, there
is, however, a
great deal of anecdotal evidence to suggest that this is indeed the
case. There is much evidence
of people going to sleep and when awakening remember something from a
dream that is a totally unique or novel idea.
Perhaps
the most famous dreamed idea was the periodic table of elements.
Mendeleev after pondering the chemical properties of various elements
for many days finally wakened from a dream with the solution mostly
intact. He explains as follows:
"I
saw in a dream a table where all the elements fell into place as
required. Awakening, I immediately wrote it down on a piece of paper.
Only in one place did a correction latter seem necessary."
Matthew
Walker tells the
story of Otto Loewi:
"Loewi
dreamed of a cleaver experiment on two frogs' hearts that would
ultimately reveal how nerve cells communicate with each other using
chemicals (neurotransmitters) released across tiny gaps that separate
them (synapses) rather than direct electrical signaling that could
happen if they were physically touching each other. So profound was
this dream that it won Loewi a Nobel Prize."
Walker gives many other
examples especially from artists and musicians. He explains Paul
McCartney credits both 'Yesterday' and 'Let It Be' as songs that came
to him in dreams and that Keith Richards recounts how
'Satisfaction' came to him in a dream. Also Penelope Lewis in her book
"The Secret World of Sleep" has this to say about it:
"This process has lead to the
creation of many works of literature, art and science, such as Mary
Shelly's Frankenstein the molecular formula of benzene, and
the invention of the lightbulb. An especially good demonstration of
somnolent creativity comes from a study of 35 musicians who not only
hear more music in their dreams than the normal man-on-the-street but
also reported that much of this (28 percent) was music they had never
heard in waking life. They had created new music in their dreams."
This of
course is all
circumstantial evidence that what is happening in dreams is
functionally to enable creativity but there a massive profusion of it
attested to by many of the greatest scientists, artist and thinkers
that
humanity has produced. For more examples see this site's page
on social creativity.
Intuition
following REM sleep.
The
other possibility is more
like problem
solving where a person
goes to sleep with a number of facts that seem incompatible or
contradictory, but on awakening has an intuition or an idea that just
pops into their head, that some how brings all those facts into a new
theory that is completely internally consistent. It suddenly explains
any seeming contradictions and smooths all the facts into being
completely
compatible.

This
is often called
a eureka moment which is widely considered to be the sudden
gaining new knowledge out of nowhere. In reality this creative
insight
usually happens over a long period of time. Each day we gather new
information and each night as we dream and that information is
integrated into an ever expanding personal knowledge. The eureka moment
itself is simply the last piece of the puzzle falling into place and
forming an entirely new theory. This theory has been growing in the
unconscious for some time and as it is completed it all becomes
conscious.
This
often takes the form of old knowledge and new information where the new
information is in conflict or dissonant with the older knowledge. The
resolution of this conflict or dissonance could be carried out by the
formation of random new connections and the strengthening of already
existing connections to produce new correlations and synthesis which
ultimately become new solutions, creative ideas and theories.
Brain processes that may induce
REM dreaming and resulting creativity.
There
is the evidence (as explained by Steven Johnsson above) that during the
REM state the whole brain is bombarded by
waves of random electrical pulses radiating from the lowest part of the
brain the brainstem.
In her book "The Secret
World of Sleep" Penelope Lewis describes two theories of how dreams may
be generated in the brain and result in creativity.
The first theory, the
activation-synthesis model.
The first is a theory by
Allan Hobson and Bob McCarly called the activation-synthesis model. She
explains this theory as follows:
"This draws on knowledge to
propose that dreams are generated in precisely the way described... [neurons
firing in the various areas of the brain that deal with vision,
hearing, (any sensory areas) and the movement area.] It turns out that chaotic firing
of neurons in the brainstem is a core characteristic of REM sleep.
Because these brainstem neurons communicate with the neocortex, their
chaotic firing could trigger responses in the primary sensory and motor
areas. The activation-synthesis model proposes that brain may combine
and synthesize these neocortical responses to create a story."
The following is from
Wikapedia:
"Neural
activity during REM sleep seems to originate in the brain stem,
especially the
pontine tegmentum and locus coeruleus. REM sleep is punctuated and
immediately
preceded by PGO (ponto-geniculo-occipital) waves, bursts of electrical
activity
originating in the brain stem. (PGO waves have long been measured
directly in
cats but not in humans because of constraints on experimentation;
however
comparable effects have been observed in humans during "phasic"
events which occur during REM sleep, and the existence of similar PGO
waves is
thus inferred.) These waves occur in clusters about every 6 seconds for
1–2
minutes during the transition from deep to paradoxical sleep. They
exhibit
their highest amplitude upon moving into the visual cortex and are a
cause of
the "rapid eye movements" in paradoxical sleep. Other muscles may also
contract under the influence of these waves."
Penelope
Lewis in her book "The Secret World of Sleep" gives some further hints
about
what is known, from various studies, to be happening in the brain
during PGO waves and REM sleep. She says:
"Even
though they [various neocortical areas] combine
together to make up a complete memory, these various neocortial areas
may not be directly interlinked. Instead the hippocampus keeps track of
such connections and forms the appropriate linkages, at least while the
memory is relatively fresh. However, communications between the
neocortex and the hippocampus is disrupted during sleep so this process
is disrupted. During REM sleep, both the hippocampus and those parts of
the neocortex which are involved in a current dream are strongly active
- but they don't appear to be in communication. Instead, responses in
the neocortex occur independently, without hippocampal input, so they
must relate to memory fragments rather than linked multi sensory
representations. Essentially, when memories that have been stored in
the neocortex are accessed or activated during REM, they remain
fragmentary instead of drawing in other aspects of the same memory to
form a complete episodic replay."
If we
examine Hobson's
later ideas in relation to these random pulses we can guess that the
random elements being
randomly inserted into REM dreams by these pulses might be the very
process of creation that brings
about knowledge stability, knowledge understanding, and knowledge
usefulness.
[Knowledge
permanence and
stability.]
By
strengthening all existing connections these brain pulses during REM
sleep could stabilize the new information by integrating it into the
old knowledge and making it less likely to be forgotten.
[Knowledge
versatility and
understanding.]
By also making new connections or reinforcing
old but weak
connections all the old knowledge the brain pulses during REM sleep
could integrate the new information with that old knowledge
with
which it shared formal features thus making new knowledge that
is both understandable and versatile.
[Knowledge
usefulness and
interconnectedness.]
By forging random, procedural, metaphorical, or analogous connections
between the new information and all the old knowledge during REM sleep
the brain could actually be creating entirely new knowledge that could
be solutions to problems that need solving, solutions to problems that
could emerge in the future or be entirely new theories
about the world. In other words this could produce creativity of every
sort.
The second theory, the brain
mechanisms of dreaming.
The second theory is by Mark
Solms called the brain mechanisms of dreaming. Lewis explains
this
theory as follows:
"His [Solms] work
suggests that the brain's
reward system, which originates in the midbrain and connects through
the ventromedial prefrontal cortex as it projects upward to the rest of
the brain, provides the basis for dreams. Lesions to
the ventromedial prefrontal region dramatically disrupt this
system." [People with such lesions lose the ability to
dream.]
Although
Solms has shown that this ventromedial forebrain area of the brain is
clearly essential for the creation of dreams his theory does not
explain the more chaotic elements and the sudden random transitions in
dreams. So this site holds that it is likely that both theories are
correct and do not have to conflict with one another. The PGO
waves from the brainstem could create disjunctions while the
ventromedial forebrain fights to place some order and narrative
sequence
between those disjunctions. The following is an excerpt from Wikapedia:
"A
mounting body of evidence suggests that dreaming and REM sleep are
dissociable states, and that dreaming is controlled
by forebrain mechanisms. Recent neuropsychological, radiological, and
pharmacological findings suggest that the cholinergic brain stem
mechanisms that control the REM state can only generate the
psychological phenomena of dreaming through the mediation of a second,
probably dopaminergic, forebrain mechanism."

Memory
melding in the furnace of dreams. Each night during REM
sleep our brains create new small ideas by fusing new information,
gained during our awake time, with previous knowledge in
storage. Sometimes these ideas come to us as intuitions the next day or
some time later. Often, however, they do not surface as intuitions for
a
long while. Instead, they sit quietly in our unconscious minds being
stitched together into a quilt like greater whole each night during REM
sleep. Eventually when they are fully formed they appear, as if from
nowhere, as a big idea
creative insight.
PROOF OF CREATION DURING AND
AFTER REM SLEEP. 
How
do we know that any of the above could be true? The
problem
with finding out what is happening in the brain when it is asleep is
the
fact that it is asleep. We cannot report what is going on because we
would have to be awake to do so. Walker explains that his colleague
Robert Stickgold designed a solution to this problem albeit an indirect
and imperfect one. Here is what they did:
"I
[previously] described
the phenomenon of sleep inertia - the carryover of the prior
sleeping brain state into wakefulness in the minutes after waking up.
We wondered whether we could turn this brief window of sleep inertia to
our experimental advantage - not by waking subjects up in the morning
and testing them, but rather by waking individuals from different
stages of NREM sleep and REM sleep throughout the night.
By
restricting the length of whatever cognitive test we performed to just
ninety seconds, we felt we could wake individuals and very quickly
test them in this transitional sleep phase. In doing so, we
could perhaps capture some of the functional properties of the sleep
stage from which the participant was woken, like capturing the vapors
of an evaporating substance and analyzing those vapors to draw
conclusions about the properties of the substance itself.
It
worked. We developed an anagram task in which the letters of real words
were scrambled. Each word was composed of five letters and the anagram
puzzles had only one correct solution (e.g., 'OSEOG' = 'GOOSE').
Participants would see the scrambled words one at a time on
the screen for just a few seconds, and they were asked to speak the
solution, if they had one, before the time ran out and the next anagram
word puzzle appeared on the screen. Each test session lasted only
ninety seconds, and we recorded how many problems the participants
correctly solved within this brief inertia period. We would then let
the participants fall back asleep.
The
subject had the task described to them before going to bed in the sleep
laboratory with electrodes placed on the head and face so that I could
measure the their sleep unfolding in real time on the monitor next
door. The participants also performed a number of trials before getting
into bed, allowing them to get familiar with the task and how it
worked. After falling asleep, I then woke the subjects up four time
throughout the night, twice from NREM sleep and early and late in the
night, twice from REM sleep, also early and late in the night.
Upon
awakening from NREM sleep, participants did not appear to be especially
creative, solving few of the anagram puzzles. But it was a different
story when I woke them from REM sleep from the dreaming phase. Overall,
problem solving abilities rocketed up, with participants solving 15 to
35 percent more puzzles when emerging from REM sleep compared with
awakening from NREM sleep or during daytime waking performance!
Moreover,
the way in which participants were solving the problems after exiting
from REM sleep was different from how they solved both when emerging
from NREM sleep and while awake during the day. The solutions simply
'popped out' following the awakenings from REM sleep, one subject told
me, though at the time, they did not know they had been in REM sleep
just prior. Solutions seemed more effortless when the brain was being
bathed by the afterglow of dream sleep. Based on response times,
solutions arrived more instantaneously following a REM sleep awakening,
relative to the slower, deliberative solutions that came when the same
individual was exiting NREM sleep or when they were awake during the
day."
So we
finally have some
empirical evidence that
links creativity with REM sleep and dreams. But this is just the
beginning. Robert Stickgold examined the way in which our stores of
related concepts (semantic knowledge) are linked together by
associations
and wondered whether these paths we usually use in searching our
memories might be bypassed or disrupted by the dreaming process to form
or prioritize more random or distant links (creative ones). He designed
another experiment to test this idea:
"Using
a standard computer test, Stickgold measured how these associative
networks of information operated following NREM sleep and REM sleep
awakenings, and during standard performance during the waking day. When
you wake the brain from NREM or measure performance during the day, the
operating principles of the brain are closely and logically connected,
just as pictured..."

This
tree represents the
normal close connections
between concepts that grow out like untangled threads. Walker continues:
"However,
wake the brain up from REM sleep and the operating algorithm was
completely different. Gone is the hierarchy of logical
associative connection. The REM sleep dreaming brain was utterly
uninterested in bland, commonsense links - the one-step-to-the-next
associations. Instead, the REM sleep brain was shortcutting the obvious
links and favoring very distantly related concepts. The logic guards
had left the REM-sleep dreaming brain. Wonderfully eclectic lunatics
were now running the associative memory asylum. [Waking] From the REM-sleep dreaming
state, almost anything goes - and the more bizarre the better the
results suggested."
The
results seemed to insist
that during REM sleep
connections were being made or reinforced that were no longer being
constrained by logic, similarity or being obvious. On the contrary, in
the REM dreaming state the brain become actively biased toward seeking
out the most distant, non obvious links in networks and between the
concepts in those networks. Matthew Walker had clearly shown there was
empirical evidence that REM dreaming was strengthening the most distant
links in brains and in doing so was initiating creativity.
SOLVING
SIMPLE PROBLEMS FOLLOWING REM SLEEP. 
But Walker wanted to prove that if a waking brain was fed the various
elements of a problem and then entered a REM dream state, the novel
connections prioritized by the REM state should provide suitable
solutions on being wakened. This
conjecture was made testable by a study designed by a colleague Jeffery
Ellenbogen. He used simple premises like A>B and
B>C the idea was to test if the relationship between
A and C could be derived i.e. = A>C:
"...we
taught participants lots of these individual premises that were nested
in a large chain of interconnections. Then we gave them tests that
assessed not just their knowledge of these individual pairs, but also
assessed whether they knew how these items connected together in the
associative chain. Only those who had slept and obtained late-morning
REM sleep, rich in dreaming, showed evidence of linking the memory
elements together (A>B>C>D>E>F,
etc.), making them capable of the most distant associative leaps (e.g.,
B>E). The very same benefit was found after daytime naps of
sixty to ninety minutes that also included REM sleep."

Clearly
the dreaming REM
sleep state was building
connections between distantly related informational elements. As Walker
says: "Our
participants went to bed with disparate pieces of the jigsaw puzzle and
woke up with the puzzle complete." This is the
difference between the retention of individual facts (informational
knowledge) and knowing what they all mean when fitted together
(integrated knowledge) (a mental map) or (cognitive
structure).
PROOF
OF REAL INSIGHT BEING GENERATED BY REM SLEEP. 
But
what about the deriving
of real insight where
the brain creates overarching knowledge and super-ordinate concepts out
of sets of information. This inspired insight is
most frequently observed in infants abstracting complex grammatical
rules in a language they must learn. It is most noticeable when they
get
it wrong as when in English there are exceptions that do not follow the
rules.
It
turns out that a study
could be devised that
could test if this creative insight is in fact generated during REM
dreaming causing both insight in dreams that are remembered and more
invisible insight that simply pops out following REM dreaming on
awakening. The study in question was designed
by Dr. Ullrtich Wagner of Germany and consisted of
participants working through hundreds of miserably laborious
number-string problems. Waker's description of the study is presented
here again from his book "Why We Sleep":
"You
will be told that you can work through these problems using specific
rules that are provided at the start of the experiment. Sneakily what
researchers do not tell you about is the existence of a hidden rule, or
shortcut that, common across all the problems. If you figure out this
embedded cheat, you can solve many more problems in a far shorter time.
I'll return to this shortcut in just a minute. After having had
participants perform hundreds of these problems, they were to return
twelve hours later and once again work through hundreds more
of these mind-numbing problems. However, at the end of this second
test session the researchers asked whether the subjects had cottoned on
to the hidden rule. Some of the participants spent the twelve-hour time
delay awake across the day while for others, that time window included
a full eight-hour night of sleep.
After
time spent awake across the day, despite the chance to consciously
deliberate on the problems as much as they desired, a rather paltry 20
percent of participants were able to extract the embedded shortcut.
Things were very different for those participants who had obtained a
full night sleep - one dressed with late with late-morning REM-rich
slumber. Almost 60 percent returned and had the 'ah-ha!' moment of
spotting the hidden cheat - which is a three fold difference in
creative insight afforded by sleep!"

As
Walker points
out it is little wonder
that we are never told to stay awake and work on a problem. Instead we
are told to "sleep on it".
This is not only true for English but also some similar saying occurs
in most other languages.
Revelations
from REM dreams.
Clearly,
when we are in REM sleep our brains are
skillfully positioning our recent biographical experiences (new
information) within the various contexts of our past accomplishments
and experiences, and so building a rich amalgam of new knowledge. This
new knowledge is exemplified by how that new information is related to
those past experiences, making them thus more understandable. It seems
as
if our brains are taking these new biological experiences and probing
how they intersect with past knowledge to relate together to produce
solutions, to both existing problems, and problems that do not at
present exist.

Solutions
and understanding.
Therefore it seems that part of the
function of REM
dreams is to make new information understandable by connecting it to
old
knowledge, and making old knowledge more understandable by connecting
it to the new information. In addition REM dreams are miraculously
creating solutions to current problems and squirreling away
possible answers to problems that might or might not eventuate in the
future. In his book "Why We Sleep" Matthew Walker explains it
as follows:
"REM
sleep, and the act of dreaming, takes that which we have learned in one
experience setting and seeks to apply it to others stored in memory."

Speculations
on how REM dreams
function. We can only guess how the brain might perform
such miraculous feats but the most obvious is that it simply tries to
fit new information into random past remembered contexts and awaits
whatever connections might be generated and thus whether new knowledge
is generated. Or the brain might be selective, selectively trying
contexts that are very distantly related and stem from similarity,
analogy or metaphor. These include all the various types of relations
between concepts or ideas such as familial, genealogical, mathematical,
functional or procedural relationships. This site's guess is that
probably both of these are true.

Creation
from REM dreams.
We can also see that totally new knowledge that refutes or contradicts
existing knowledge could also be generated in this manner. Such knew
knowledge could include new speculation, new conjecture and totally new
theories not to mention totally new combinations of forms that create
totally new works of art, music, and craftsmanship.
WHAT WE DREAM ABOUT
MATTERS. 
The content of each dream matters.
It might seem obvious that
the content of each dream has a function and a purpose but scientists
need to prove it. After all in the past people believed that dreams
predicted the future. Not
surprisingly Walker
wanted to
prove, beyond a shadow of a doubt, that it was the content of those
dreams that was making
the
difference and not just the dream state itself that was
causing the
creativity. He explains how this was managed:
"Though
such a claim has long been made, it took the advent of of virtual
reality for us to prove as much...
Enter
my collaborator Robert Stickgold, who designed a clever experiment in
which participants explored a computerized virtual reality maze. During
the initial learning session, he would start participants off from
different random locations within the virtual maze and ask them to
navigate their way out through exploratory trial and error. To aid
their learning Stickgold placed unique objects, such as a Christmas
tree, to act as orientation or anchor points at specific locations
within the virtual maze.

Almost
a hundred research participants explored the maze during the first
learning session. Thereafter, half of them took a ninety minute nap,
while the other half remained awake and watched video, all
monitored with electrodes placed on their heads and face.

Throughout
the ninety minute epoch, Stickgold would occasionally wake the napping
individuals and ask them about the content of any dream they were
having, or for the group that remained awake, ask them to report any
particular thoughts that were going through their minds at the time.
Following the ninety-minute period, and after another hour or so to
overcome sleep inertia in those who had napped, everyone was dropped
back into the virtual maze and tested once more to see if their
performance was any better than during the initial learning.
It
should come as no surprise by now that those participants who took a
nap showed superior memory performance on the maze task. They could
locate the navigation clues with ease, finding their way around and out
of the maze faster than those who had not slept. The novel result,
however, was the difference that dreaming made. Participants who slept
and reported dreaming elements of the maze and themes around
experiences clearly related to to it, showed almost ten times more
improvement in their task performance upon awakening than those who who
slept just as much, and also dreamed, but did not dream of maze-related
experiences.
As
in his earlier studies, Stickgold found that the dreams of these
super-navigators were not a precise replay of the initial learning
experience while awake. For example, one of the participant's dream
report stated: 'I was thinking about the maze and kinda having people
as checkpoints, I guess and then that led me to think about when I went
on this trip a few years ago and we went to see these bat caves, and
they're kind of like maze like.' There were no bats in
Stickgold's
virtual maze, nor were there any other people or checkpoints. Clearly
the dreaming brain was not simply recapitulating or re-creating exactly
what happened to them in the maze. Rather the dream algorithm was
cherry
picking salient fragments of the prior learning experience, and then
attempting to place those new experiences within the back catalogue of
preexisting knowledge."
As
Walker explains,
when we are in REM sleep our brains picks salient fragments of past
experience and melds them together with our recent biographical
experiences (new
information) to form new ideas or knowledge. Alternatively, during REM
sleep, our brains may drop this new information within various contexts
of our past accomplishments
and experiences, to test if it can work in those various contexts and
thus produce original creative ideas. In this way new information is
either accommodated or assimilated into the cognitive structure thus,
forming as it does, better understanding of stored knowledge, solutions
to existing or non existing problems, and original, unique
creations. These are all forms of new knowledge that seem to just
suddenly emerge in our minds.
Controlling
your dreams.
LUCIDITY. 
Lucidity
has several meanings but most often it is used to describe gaining
volitional control of what an individual is dreaming and the ability to
manipulate that experience. For instance an individual might decide to
fly or even solve a problem. Although claims of such abilities have
been around for a while they do not go back far in our
history.

This
being the case and having little idea as to how such a phenomenon as
lucidity might
be tested led to much skepticism as to whether this could be possible
or
was just stories people told. However as Walker explains:
"Four years ago, an
ingenious experiment removed all such doubt. Scientists placed lucid
dreamers in an MRI scanner. While awake, these participants first
clenched their left and right hand, over and over. Researchers took
snapshots of the brain activity, allowing them to define the precise
brain areas controlling each hand of each individual.

The participants were
allowed to fall asleep in the MRI scanner entering REM sleep where they
could dream. During REM sleep, however, all voluntary muscles are
paralyzed, preventing the dreamer from acting out ongoing mental
experience. Yet, the muscles that control the eyes are spared from this
paralysis, and give this stage of sleep its frenetic name. Lucid
dreamers were able to take advantage of this ocular freedom,
communicating with the researchers through eye movements. Pre-defined
eye movements would therefore inform the researchers of the nature of
the lucid dream (e.g.,the participant made three deliberate leftward
eye movements when they gained lucid dream control, two rightward eye
movements before clenching their right hand etc)...

When participants signaled
the beginning of the lucid dream state, the scientists began taking MRI
pictures of the brain activity. Soon after, the sleeping participants
signaled their intent to dream about moving their left hand, then
their right, alternating over and over, just as they did when awake.
Their hands were not physically moving - they could not, due to the
REM-sleep paralysis. But they were moving in the dream.
At least that was the
subjective claim from the participants upon awakening. The results of
the MRI scans objectively proved they were not lying. The same regions
of the brain that were active during physical right and left voluntary
hand movements observed while the individuals were awake
similarly lit up in corresponding ways during times when the
lucid participants signaled they were clenching their hands while
dreaming.
There could be no question.
Scientists had gained objective proof that lucid dreamers can
control when and what they dream while they are dreaming."
Walker
is quite aware this
sounds improbable but
as he says the proof is indisputable.
"Non-lucid
dreamers find it difficult to believe that such deliberate eye
movements are possible while someone is asleep, but watch a lucid
dreamer do it a number of times and it is impossible to deny."
Life
long learning, and the functions of sleeping and dreaming.
As
stated above sleep and dreaming help us with
various processes of learning. NREM sleep is where new information is
classified as important or unimportant. It is where that information is
consolidated and reinforced if it is classified as important, and
deleted, or left to wither, if
it is classified as unimportant. REM sleep prepares
us for the unknown,
and enables us to be the human problem solvers and theorists
that
gradually uncover the secrets of the universe.
But
perhaps in doing all this
sleep and dreaming also
enable us to
desire to learn this that and the other and to enjoy each type of
learning.
These joys and desires are contingent on two things.
Firstly the
enjoyment of learning and the desire to learn is
contingent on how well we learn anything. There is a joy in
knowing and
expanding our understanding of the patterns of the universe. But this
can only exist if we have learned
those things well. Sleep and dreaming ensure that we are able to learn
things well.
Secondly the enjoyment of
learning and the desire to
learn is contingent on and how useful, what we have learned, is in our
lives. The joy of being able to help ourselves through life's
journey with what we know is made more enjoyable and desirable by
being useful to us. Sleep and dreaming also help to make what we learn
useful to us on a continuing basis.
It
makes sense then that we should come to desire to
learn and enjoy learning. If we get enough sleep we will will both
enjoy learning and desire to learn so much we will want to continuously
learn and thus
continue to learn all the days of our lives.
|