Cognitive
structure stage seven (4 to about 6 or 7 years of age)
Multiple causes and
diverging possibilities for reality.
By
the age of about 4 most children have become emotionally invested in
becoming accurate in their expectations of outcomes. This enables the
possibility of being able to entertain the idea that there may
be
many possible paths through reality and that the same outcome can be in
response to very different causes. Greenspan
and Shanker in their book "The First Idea" elaborate on this phenomenon:
"From
simple causal thinking children progress to recognizing multiple causes
often experiencing a rapid growth in this capacity between four and
seven years of age. If someone won't play, instead of just concluding,
'She hates me,' the child can say, 'Maybe she has someone else she
wants to play with today. Maybe her mother is making her come home
after school.' She can set up multiple hypotheses. Or, 'Maybe she
doesn't want to play with me because I have always played Nintendo.
Maybe if I offer to do something else she'll want to come over.' The
child is now becoming a multiple-cause thinker in many contexts. In
school she can now look at multiple reasons for the Civil War or why a
storybook character is upset. With peers she can compare two friends -
'I like Sally better than Stephanie because she has great toys.'
Multiple-cause thinking enables her to engage in 'triangular' thinking.
At home if mother is annoyed, the child can try to make her mother
jealous by going to her father and being coy with him. She can figure
out how a character in a book pretended to like vegetables so that she
could get desert."
"To
learn multiple-cause thinking a child not only needs to have learned
the earlier levels discussed above, she needs to be able to invest
emotionally in more than one possibility. For example she may not be
able to consider a second friend as a possible play partner if she is
too dependent on the first friend. She may believe that she will 'lose'
that friend unless she plays only with her. She may not be able to woo
her father into playing with her or even consider this possibility if
she is too anxious about losing her mother."
Flexibility of theories.
This
possibility of having different causes presenting the same outcome
naturally leads to the possibility of having competing or alternative
theories that predict different outcomes. From there it is a small step
to envisioning alternative theories that predict the same outcome. When
such testing as the child is able to perform is unable to eliminate one
of those theories, the child can be left with the quandary of two
theories either which might be correct. As the child begins to work out
all the possibilities involved, theory building becomes very complex,
and both more accurate but more tentative. Perhaps one is true or maybe
both are independently true or maybe both theories need to be combined
to produce the outcome or effect. In other words one cause may produce
the effect, each cause may independently produce the same effect, or some
combination of the two or more causes may be necessary to produce the
effect.
In this way the child for
the first time is able to hold
alternative possibilities or theories in his/her mind without having to
declare a winner. One may be right the other may be right both may
independently be right or some combination of both or more causes may
be necessary. The child's ability hold these possibilities in mind leads
naturally to the creation of ever more complex hypotheses to be tested.
The free floating islands of knowledge we have called reality patterns
at this stage become overlapping with far more numerous connections
which at the same time become less rigid and more tentative. This in
turn allows for increasing flexibility in thinking. Greenspan
and Shanker have this to say:
"The
child becomes a more flexible thinker as a result of multiple cause
thinking. Eventually she comes to understand more intricate
plots
in literature, the multiple causes for historical events, and a
physical phenomenon that requires scientific explanation. Therefore
multiple-cause thinking constitutes a higher level of reflective
thinking in all spheres."
Social dynamics.
The
ability to create multiple hypotheses and to test them leads to many
sorts of new knowledge. Perhaps the most important of these new bits of
knowledge is the ability to attribute several different motivations in
others to the same outcome. This enables a far greater understanding of
social dynamics than was previously possible. Greenspan
and Shanker put it like this:
"The
child can now also understand her own family dynamics through
relationships among different people rather than just whether her own
needs are met. For example, it's not simply a question of whether her
mother is paying enough attention to her but how she compares with a
sibling when they both want mother's attention. Similarly, she can
negotiate social triangles and figure out that another child may prefer
to play with someone else on a given day without necessarily disliking
her.
Intelligence,
social skills and the expanding sense of self.
The
child seeing multiple causes in all thing is now able to see multiple
causes in him/herself. The child is able to select multiple facets of
his/her own self to make use of, just as is possible with external
objects. Greenspan
and Shanker put it like this:
As
the child's intelligence and social skills are advancing her sense of
self is expanding to include new horizons as well. She can now begin to
look at herself as competitive and needy, mischievous and
funny (multiple dimensions) all at the same time. Her consciousness is
gradually developing an awareness of how she can employ these different
facets of herself in a variety of contexts - whether it's to compete
with her sibling or recruit a new friend at school."
|